Why Do I Need You?

Download Subtitles

Transcript

0:00:04 > 0:00:06What does a brain need to be healthy?

0:00:08 > 0:00:11Well, it needs nutrients from the food you eat,

0:00:11 > 0:00:14it needs oxygen from your blood, plenty of water.

0:00:14 > 0:00:18But there's something else, something equally as important.

0:00:18 > 0:00:20It needs other people.

0:00:20 > 0:00:25CHEERING

0:00:31 > 0:00:34Human beings are extremely social creatures.

0:00:36 > 0:00:39We come together, we team up,

0:00:39 > 0:00:43we share moments of intense joy and disappointment.

0:00:46 > 0:00:49We don't just seek out other people to have a good time.

0:00:49 > 0:00:53Your brain function depends on the social web that you're in.

0:00:53 > 0:00:58Your neurons require other people's neurons to thrive and survive.

0:00:59 > 0:01:03I want to show you how our brains are fundamentally wired

0:01:03 > 0:01:04to work together...

0:01:07 > 0:01:10..how this social network that envelops us

0:01:10 > 0:01:13from birth is vital for our survival.

0:01:17 > 0:01:19Yeah? OK!

0:01:19 > 0:01:22Understanding how brains deal with each other allows us

0:01:22 > 0:01:28to understand what bonds our species, driving us to help

0:01:28 > 0:01:32one another and what makes us hate...

0:01:33 > 0:01:36..what allows acts of human violence.

0:01:37 > 0:01:41It helps us to make sense of our past

0:01:41 > 0:01:43and holds the key to our future.

0:01:56 > 0:02:02There are seven billion people living today - seven billion brains

0:02:02 > 0:02:06moving, choosing, acting,

0:02:06 > 0:02:10believing and connecting with other brains.

0:02:12 > 0:02:16Brains are traditionally studied in isolation but, in fact,

0:02:16 > 0:02:20much of the circuitry of the brain has to do with other brains.

0:02:20 > 0:02:22We're fundamentally social creatures

0:02:22 > 0:02:26and our society is a complex web of interaction.

0:02:26 > 0:02:31On any normal day, we intersect with an enormous number of people.

0:02:33 > 0:02:36'Our lives are built on these intersections,

0:02:36 > 0:02:42'not just between us and our family and friends and work colleagues,

0:02:42 > 0:02:45'but also between them and the people they meet.'

0:02:51 > 0:02:54'Even the most basic encounter...'

0:02:54 > 0:02:56like getting a cup of coffee...

0:02:56 > 0:02:59'..relies on trust with a stranger.'

0:02:59 > 0:03:01- Could I get a latte, please? - Definitely.- Thanks.

0:03:04 > 0:03:10Everywhere we look, we see complex social interactions, relationships

0:03:10 > 0:03:16forming and breaking, bonds of love and support, social networking.

0:03:18 > 0:03:22'We clump into large groups to share our knowledge.'

0:03:22 > 0:03:25You've got all these random spots in your brain that get

0:03:25 > 0:03:27wired up into an associative neural network.

0:03:27 > 0:03:32'We work to impress each other and we swap ideas.'

0:03:32 > 0:03:34I'll stick around for any questions that anyone has. Thank you.

0:03:34 > 0:03:37CLAPPING

0:03:38 > 0:03:41'Most research looks at one brain at a time,

0:03:41 > 0:03:45'but that misses the fact that a great deal of our brain activity

0:03:45 > 0:03:49'is dedicated to communicating with each other,

0:03:49 > 0:03:50'interpreting each other.'

0:03:51 > 0:03:55'Our social drive is deeply rooted in our neural circuitry.'

0:04:00 > 0:04:02Take a look at this film from the 1940s.

0:04:02 > 0:04:04What do you see happening here?

0:04:05 > 0:04:10Is this just a simple animation of some shapes or something more?

0:04:10 > 0:04:14Do you see a chase, a fight, a love story?

0:04:16 > 0:04:20The big one seems to be pushing the little one around.

0:04:20 > 0:04:25It seems like the two triangles are in a little bit of a squabble.

0:04:25 > 0:04:26There are relationships here

0:04:26 > 0:04:29in terms of one is more dominant than the other.

0:04:31 > 0:04:34Back in the 1940s, psychologists Fritz Heider

0:04:34 > 0:04:39and Marianne Simmel created this film as part of an experiment.

0:04:39 > 0:04:42- The ball doesn't seem to want to be in there.- It's freaking out.

0:04:42 > 0:04:45It's scared. It looks like a trap to me.

0:04:46 > 0:04:49It looks like the smaller triangle is being shut out and, like,

0:04:49 > 0:04:52trying to peer in.

0:04:52 > 0:04:56- They are paired in a way that seems friendly.- Yeah.

0:04:56 > 0:04:58- This is really fun.- It's fun, right?

0:04:59 > 0:05:02'What Heider and Simmel found, as I did,

0:05:02 > 0:05:07'is how easy it is to look at moving shapes and to see meaning

0:05:07 > 0:05:12'and motives and emotion, all in the form of a social narrative.'

0:05:14 > 0:05:16I kind of get the sense that they're cats and dogs.

0:05:16 > 0:05:18It seemed like the big one might have been,

0:05:18 > 0:05:20like, his dad or something.

0:05:20 > 0:05:22Just call it more of, like, a mating ritual -

0:05:22 > 0:05:25two competitors going for one possible mate.

0:05:28 > 0:05:31These are just shapes on a screen but we can't help

0:05:31 > 0:05:34but tell stories about them. Why?

0:05:34 > 0:05:37It's because our brains are so primed for social interaction

0:05:37 > 0:05:41that we look for intention in relationships all around us.

0:05:43 > 0:05:45One way we navigate the social world

0:05:45 > 0:05:49is by judging other people's intentions.

0:05:49 > 0:05:52Is she is trying to be helpful?

0:05:54 > 0:05:56Are we a trustworthy team?

0:05:57 > 0:06:00Our brains are good at making these sorts of judgements

0:06:00 > 0:06:03and we do it constantly

0:06:03 > 0:06:07but do we learn this skill from life experience or are we born with it?

0:06:10 > 0:06:13To figure out which one it is, I've invited over some people who

0:06:13 > 0:06:16don't have much experience with the world.

0:06:16 > 0:06:18SHE CRIES

0:06:18 > 0:06:20I've invited them to a puppet show.

0:06:22 > 0:06:26These babies are all under 12 months old.

0:06:26 > 0:06:30They're just beginning to explore the world around them.

0:06:30 > 0:06:33You could say they're all a little short on life experience.

0:06:35 > 0:06:37We decided to run a simple experiment

0:06:37 > 0:06:39developed at Yale University.

0:06:44 > 0:06:46Here's a duck struggling to open a box.

0:06:51 > 0:06:53One bear helps the duck.

0:06:59 > 0:07:01The other is mean to the duck.

0:07:05 > 0:07:08OK, Booey. Here you go. There are two puppets.

0:07:08 > 0:07:12'When the show's over, I let the babies choose a bear to play with.'

0:07:15 > 0:07:17Yeah, OK. Is that the one you like? All right.

0:07:17 > 0:07:21'Almost every one of them chooses the bear that's been kind.'

0:07:23 > 0:07:25'These babies can't walk or talk

0:07:25 > 0:07:30'and yet they already have the tools to make judgements about others.'

0:07:32 > 0:07:33Yeah? OK!

0:07:35 > 0:07:37It's often assumed that trust is something that we

0:07:37 > 0:07:40learn from our experience in the world, but these experiments

0:07:40 > 0:07:44demonstrate that, even as babies, we come equipped with

0:07:44 > 0:07:47social antennae for feeling our way through the world.

0:07:47 > 0:07:51The brain comes with inborn instincts for figuring out

0:07:51 > 0:07:53who's trustworthy and who's not.

0:07:53 > 0:07:54BABY GURGLES

0:07:55 > 0:08:00As we grow, our social challenges become even more subtle

0:08:00 > 0:08:01and complex.

0:08:03 > 0:08:07Understanding others is one of the most demanding operations

0:08:07 > 0:08:08that our brains perform.

0:08:11 > 0:08:16They have to interpret words and, more than that, inflection,

0:08:16 > 0:08:18facial expressions, body language.

0:08:21 > 0:08:22Does she like me?

0:08:24 > 0:08:26Is he interested in what I'm saying?

0:08:28 > 0:08:29Do they want my help?

0:08:31 > 0:08:37Society runs on our ability to read each other's social signals.

0:08:39 > 0:08:44Take that ability away and the world becomes a very strange place.

0:08:52 > 0:08:56Car enthusiast John Robeson has always struggled to read

0:08:56 > 0:08:57other people.

0:08:59 > 0:09:03When I was a little boy, I was bullied and rejected by

0:09:03 > 0:09:07other kids, and that didn't happen with machines, you know.

0:09:07 > 0:09:12I could stand by a tractor in my grandparents' farm

0:09:12 > 0:09:16and I could learn how to adjust it and it wouldn't tease me

0:09:16 > 0:09:18or do anything bad, it wouldn't run away, it would

0:09:18 > 0:09:20always be there and I could count on it.

0:09:20 > 0:09:24And I guess I learned to make friends with

0:09:24 > 0:09:29the machines before I learned how to make friends with other people.

0:09:29 > 0:09:32In time, John's affinity for technology took him

0:09:32 > 0:09:35to places his bullies could only dream of.

0:09:36 > 0:09:40By 21, he was a roadie for the band Kiss.

0:09:40 > 0:09:44This was me, back with Kiss in the '70s.

0:09:44 > 0:09:47I'm older and fatter and stuff. I don't look the same any more.

0:09:50 > 0:09:53Surrounded by legendary rock and roll excess,

0:09:53 > 0:09:57his outlook remained different from other people's.

0:09:57 > 0:10:00People would come up to me all the time and they would say,

0:10:00 > 0:10:03"What's this guy like?" or, "What's that guy like?"

0:10:04 > 0:10:06I would say, "Yeah, their stage set-up,

0:10:06 > 0:10:10"they had Sunn 2000S bass amps," or, "Gene played Sunn Coliseums,

0:10:10 > 0:10:13"and we had seven bass amps chained together,

0:10:13 > 0:10:16"we had 2,200 watts in the bass system for that."

0:10:17 > 0:10:18But I maybe couldn't tell you

0:10:18 > 0:10:21the first thing about the musicians who sang through them.

0:10:24 > 0:10:28Now I realise that shows that I did kind of live in

0:10:28 > 0:10:32a different world all those years - a world of machines and equipment.

0:10:35 > 0:10:41When he was 40, John was diagnosed with Asperger's - a form of autism.

0:10:48 > 0:10:51Many regions of the brain are engaged during

0:10:51 > 0:10:56social interaction, but in autism, that brain activity isn't seen

0:10:56 > 0:11:01as strongly, and that's paralleled by diminished social skills.

0:11:02 > 0:11:08I didn't really understand that there were complex messages

0:11:08 > 0:11:13in faces until I was well into adulthood and learned about autism.

0:11:15 > 0:11:20I knew that people could display signs of crazed anger..

0:11:22 > 0:11:28..but if you asked about more subtle expressions, you know,

0:11:28 > 0:11:31I think you're sweet and I wonder what you're hiding,

0:11:31 > 0:11:35or, I'd really like to do that, or, I wish you'd do this or that, I...

0:11:36 > 0:11:39I had no idea about things like that.

0:11:45 > 0:11:48But then came a transforming moment in John's life.

0:11:49 > 0:11:54In 2008, he was invited to Harvard Medical School to take part

0:11:54 > 0:12:00in an experiment on his brain - overseen by Dr Alvaro Pascual-Leone.

0:12:02 > 0:12:05It was an attempt to try to understand

0:12:05 > 0:12:07how activity in one area

0:12:07 > 0:12:12affects activity in another area and how that affects behaviour.

0:12:14 > 0:12:17The experiment was only meant to help the scientists gain

0:12:17 > 0:12:19greater knowledge about the autistic brain.

0:12:21 > 0:12:24But then, something unexpected happened.

0:12:27 > 0:12:32John was given transcranial magnetic stimulation, or TMS.

0:12:32 > 0:12:36Magnetic coils were placed next to his head to generate

0:12:36 > 0:12:40minute electrical currents in the brain and alter its activity.

0:12:42 > 0:12:46The researchers targeted different regions of John's brain to see

0:12:46 > 0:12:50whether interfering with his brain activity had

0:12:50 > 0:12:56- any effect on his behaviour.- They would test me after the session.

0:12:56 > 0:12:59I would go home, kind of not knowing what to expect.

0:13:00 > 0:13:02At first, there was no result,

0:13:02 > 0:13:06but then they targeted the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex,

0:13:06 > 0:13:11a region involved in flexible thinking and abstraction,

0:13:11 > 0:13:14and something dramatic happened.

0:13:14 > 0:13:15Somehow, I became different.

0:13:19 > 0:13:24He contacted us, very excited, to say, you know,

0:13:24 > 0:13:27"The effects of the stimulation seemed to have unlocked

0:13:27 > 0:13:30"something and the effects are still lasting

0:13:30 > 0:13:33"and I now can do things that I could never do!"

0:13:38 > 0:13:44After TMS, I was able to sort of read signals from other people

0:13:44 > 0:13:46and understand what was going on.

0:13:48 > 0:13:53So I listened to that, fascinated by it, and thought, OK, well, whatever.

0:13:53 > 0:13:55It'll go away. But it didn't.

0:13:55 > 0:13:58It actually remained something that had really

0:13:58 > 0:14:00fundamentally changed in him.

0:14:03 > 0:14:06Somehow, and entirely accidentally,

0:14:06 > 0:14:10the TMS had unlocked a whole new world for John.

0:14:10 > 0:14:11A vegetable sandwich to bring home...

0:14:11 > 0:14:16'I'd be tempted to say I couldn't read people and now I can,

0:14:16 > 0:14:18'but that's not really true.'

0:14:18 > 0:14:21- OK, how about a full-sized... One of them?- Sure.

0:14:21 > 0:14:25It's more accurate to say I had no idea there were these

0:14:25 > 0:14:28messages emanating from other people.

0:14:30 > 0:14:34'TMS showed me those messages, and now that

0:14:34 > 0:14:38'I'm aware that they're out there, everything I do is different.'

0:14:39 > 0:14:42All of a sudden, you can walk around and engage the world...

0:14:43 > 0:14:45'..and it's a big, big thing.'

0:14:46 > 0:14:47OK, thanks.

0:14:50 > 0:14:53We don't know exactly what happened neurobiologically

0:14:53 > 0:14:55but I think it now offers the opportunity for us

0:14:55 > 0:15:00to understand what behavioural modifications, what interventions

0:15:00 > 0:15:04might be possible to learn from him that we can then teach others.

0:15:07 > 0:15:11John's transformation is a reminder that all

0:15:11 > 0:15:13the activities of the human brain,

0:15:13 > 0:15:18including the subtle interplay of emotions and relationships,

0:15:18 > 0:15:21are rooted in the detailed patterns

0:15:21 > 0:15:24of trillions of electrochemical signals.

0:15:32 > 0:15:34Somehow, humans can look at each other

0:15:34 > 0:15:38and study the arrangement of facial muscles

0:15:38 > 0:15:42and then process that information into an understanding of

0:15:42 > 0:15:45other people's thoughts and emotions.

0:15:47 > 0:15:52It's an astonishing skill because the cues are so subtle

0:15:52 > 0:15:55and the processing is so rapid

0:15:55 > 0:15:59that the whole operation runs under your radar.

0:16:01 > 0:16:06It only takes 33 milliseconds for your brain to process basic

0:16:06 > 0:16:12information about someone's facial expression and start reacting to it.

0:16:12 > 0:16:15So we're going to put one electrode right above your eyebrow...

0:16:15 > 0:16:16'So how does it do that?'

0:16:16 > 0:16:19..and the other right on your cheek. There we go. Great.

0:16:22 > 0:16:27'I've invited a group of people to run an experiment.

0:16:27 > 0:16:30'I've wired up to a machine that measures movements in their

0:16:30 > 0:16:35'facial muscles and I've asked them to look at photographs of faces.'

0:16:44 > 0:16:47When participants are looking at a photograph with a smile

0:16:47 > 0:16:51or a frown, we see this activity on the graph,

0:16:51 > 0:16:54which indicates that their own facial muscles are moving.

0:16:55 > 0:16:56Why?

0:16:59 > 0:17:02Well, it turns out that they are automatically mirroring,

0:17:02 > 0:17:06with their own faces, the expressions that they're seeing.

0:17:09 > 0:17:14- That was fun, right, the last one? - Yeah.- Yeah, that was a fun test!

0:17:14 > 0:17:16But what purpose does this mirroring serve?

0:17:18 > 0:17:21I've invited a second group of people.

0:17:21 > 0:17:25They're similar to the first group except for one thing.

0:17:32 > 0:17:36This is the most lethal neurotoxin on the planet.

0:17:36 > 0:17:39If you were to ingest even a fraction of this,

0:17:39 > 0:17:42your brain could no longer tell your muscles how to contract,

0:17:42 > 0:17:44and you would die of total paralysis.

0:17:44 > 0:17:48So it seems unlikely that anyone would pay to have this

0:17:48 > 0:17:50injected into themselves, but they do.

0:17:52 > 0:17:55This is known as botulinum toxin or Botox.

0:17:57 > 0:18:00If you put in your forehead muscles, it paralyses them

0:18:00 > 0:18:02to reduce wrinkling.

0:18:04 > 0:18:07But there's a less well-known side-effect.

0:18:11 > 0:18:16When our participants with Botox went through the same tests,

0:18:16 > 0:18:20their facial muscles responded less. No surprise there.

0:18:20 > 0:18:24But replicating an experiment out of Duke University,

0:18:24 > 0:18:27we had both groups look at facial expressions

0:18:27 > 0:18:32and now they were asked to choose the word that best described

0:18:32 > 0:18:34the emotion they were seeing.

0:18:39 > 0:18:41Panic.

0:18:41 > 0:18:43Panicked.

0:18:45 > 0:18:46Upset.

0:18:48 > 0:18:53On average, the Botox group was worse at identifying

0:18:53 > 0:18:54the emotions correctly.

0:18:56 > 0:18:57Sceptical?

0:18:57 > 0:19:01It seems that the lack of feedback from their facial muscles

0:19:01 > 0:19:05impairs their ability to read other people.

0:19:05 > 0:19:10The paralysed faces of Botox users not only makes it hard for us

0:19:10 > 0:19:12to tell what THEY'RE feeling,

0:19:12 > 0:19:17those same frozen muscles make it hard for THEM to read US.

0:19:18 > 0:19:21And that tells us something.

0:19:21 > 0:19:24When I'm happy or sad, part of that feeling

0:19:24 > 0:19:29relies on the unconscious feedback from muscles in my face.

0:19:29 > 0:19:32And our social brains take advantage of that,

0:19:32 > 0:19:36so when we're trying to understand what someone else is feeling,

0:19:36 > 0:19:38we try on their facial expression.

0:19:41 > 0:19:46This automatic mirroring of expressions is just one way

0:19:46 > 0:19:49in which we understand others.

0:19:49 > 0:19:52The brain also has a deeper way,

0:19:52 > 0:19:55one that's best explained at the movies.

0:20:03 > 0:20:05One ticket, please.

0:20:06 > 0:20:08Thank you.

0:20:10 > 0:20:12When we go to the movie theatre,

0:20:12 > 0:20:15we know full well that it's make-believe.

0:20:15 > 0:20:19The people on the screen are just acting.

0:20:19 > 0:20:25And yet, we still react. We gasp and flinch and cry.

0:20:25 > 0:20:26Why do we fall for it?

0:20:33 > 0:20:36To understand why we care about other people getting hurt,

0:20:36 > 0:20:40we need to understand what happens in your brain when you get hurt.

0:20:40 > 0:20:41So imagine that somebody

0:20:41 > 0:20:44were to stab your hand with a syringe needle.

0:20:44 > 0:20:47That activates a network of areas in your brain that we call

0:20:47 > 0:20:50the pain matrix.

0:20:52 > 0:20:56There's no single spot in the brain where pain is processed.

0:20:57 > 0:21:01Instead, the perception of pain arises from several different

0:21:01 > 0:21:03areas networking together.

0:21:03 > 0:21:06Strangely enough,

0:21:06 > 0:21:10this pain matrix is at the heart of how we connect with others.

0:21:13 > 0:21:16Now, when you watch someone else get stabbed,

0:21:16 > 0:21:19your pain matrix becomes activated.

0:21:20 > 0:21:24Not the parts that tell you you've actually been touched,

0:21:24 > 0:21:28but the parts involved in the emotional experience of pain.

0:21:28 > 0:21:33In other words, watching someone else in pain and being in pain

0:21:33 > 0:21:38use the same neural machinery and that's the basis of empathy.

0:21:47 > 0:21:52To empathise with another person is to literally feel their pain.

0:21:55 > 0:21:59You run a compelling simulation of what it would be like

0:21:59 > 0:22:02if you were in that situation.

0:22:02 > 0:22:07And our capacity to do this is why stories and movies and novels

0:22:07 > 0:22:12are so absorbing and why they're so pervasive across human culture

0:22:12 > 0:22:17because whether it's about total strangers or made-up characters,

0:22:17 > 0:22:23you experience their agony and their ecstasy, you fluidly become them

0:22:23 > 0:22:27and live their lives and stand in their vantage points.

0:22:30 > 0:22:33You can tell yourself that the stories aren't real,

0:22:33 > 0:22:37but some neurons deep in your brain can't tell the difference.

0:22:48 > 0:22:52Our capacity to feel another person's pain is part of what

0:22:52 > 0:22:55makes us so good at taking other people's perspective,

0:22:55 > 0:22:59to step out of our shoes and into their shoes, neurally speaking.

0:23:02 > 0:23:05We can't help but connect with others.

0:23:05 > 0:23:09We're hotwired to be extremely social creatures.

0:23:09 > 0:23:14And that raises a question - what would happen

0:23:14 > 0:23:17if the brain were starved of human contact?

0:23:24 > 0:23:28In 2009, peace activist Sarah Shourd

0:23:28 > 0:23:32and her two companions were hiking in the mountains of northern Iraq,

0:23:32 > 0:23:35an area that was at the time peaceful,

0:23:35 > 0:23:39but they accidentally strayed into Iran and they were arrested.

0:23:41 > 0:23:45They pulled us apart and threw us in separate cells and slammed the door.

0:23:45 > 0:23:47And um...

0:23:47 > 0:23:53That was the beginning of the next 410 days of my life in that cell.

0:23:59 > 0:24:03In the early weeks and really months of solitary confinement,

0:24:03 > 0:24:06you're reduced to an animal-like state.

0:24:06 > 0:24:08I mean, you are an animal, in a cage.

0:24:14 > 0:24:18And the majority of your hours are pacing,

0:24:23 > 0:24:27and the animal-like state sort of eventually

0:24:27 > 0:24:30transformed into a more plant-like state.

0:24:32 > 0:24:37When your mind starts to slow down and your thoughts become repetitive.

0:24:44 > 0:24:46Your brain turns on itself

0:24:46 > 0:24:53and it becomes the source of your worst pain and your worst torture.

0:25:00 > 0:25:02I would relive every detail of my life.

0:25:04 > 0:25:08And eventually, you run out of memories and you've told them

0:25:08 > 0:25:12all to yourself so many times and it doesn't take that long.

0:25:12 > 0:25:17Extreme social deprivation causes deep psychological pain.

0:25:17 > 0:25:21Without interaction, the brain suffers.

0:25:22 > 0:25:26Solitary confinement is designed to eat away at

0:25:26 > 0:25:29and really attack what essentially makes us human.

0:25:33 > 0:25:37Sarah's brain used the scant sensory information it had

0:25:37 > 0:25:40to construct a reality.

0:25:41 > 0:25:44The sun would come in at a certain time of day at an angle

0:25:44 > 0:25:49through my window and all of the little dust particles in my cell

0:25:49 > 0:25:51were illuminated by the sun.

0:25:53 > 0:25:56I saw all of those particles of dust as being

0:25:56 > 0:25:59other human beings occupying the planet.

0:25:59 > 0:26:03And they were in the stream of life, they were interacting, they

0:26:03 > 0:26:07were bouncing off one another, they were doing something collective.

0:26:11 > 0:26:15And I saw myself as off in a corner, you know,

0:26:15 > 0:26:18walled off by myself, out of the stream of life.

0:26:24 > 0:26:30In September 2010, after 410 days in solitary confinement,

0:26:30 > 0:26:33Sarah was finally released and allowed to rejoin the world.

0:26:36 > 0:26:41But for a long time, she suffered from extreme post-traumatic stress.

0:26:44 > 0:26:48The philosopher Martin Heidegger said we can't talk about being,

0:26:48 > 0:26:51we can only talk about being in the world.

0:26:51 > 0:26:56In other words, the world around you is a part of who you are.

0:26:58 > 0:27:01In a vacuum, you lose your sense of self.

0:27:05 > 0:27:08It's not easy for science to study people

0:27:08 > 0:27:11while they're experiencing solitary confinement,

0:27:11 > 0:27:14but a simple experiment designed by neuroscientist

0:27:14 > 0:27:16Naomi Eisenberger can give us

0:27:16 > 0:27:21an insight into what's happening in the brain when we feel excluded.

0:27:24 > 0:27:27It's based on a game of catch.

0:27:29 > 0:27:33While volunteers played a computer game of catch, Eisenberger

0:27:33 > 0:27:35and her team scanned their brains.

0:27:37 > 0:27:41The volunteers thought the other characters were controlled by other

0:27:41 > 0:27:45participants, but in fact, they were just part of a computer program.

0:27:47 > 0:27:50At first, the other characters played nicely,

0:27:50 > 0:27:54but after a while, they'd cut the volunteer out of the game.

0:27:54 > 0:27:57And simply play between themselves.

0:27:59 > 0:28:05She found that being left out of the game activated the pain matrix.

0:28:09 > 0:28:12Not getting the ball might seem insignificant,

0:28:12 > 0:28:17but to the brain, social rejection is so meaningful that it hurts.

0:28:22 > 0:28:25But that pain, in turn, is useful.

0:28:28 > 0:28:31It pushes us in the direction of bonding with others.

0:28:37 > 0:28:40We all seek out alliances.

0:28:40 > 0:28:44We join with friends, with family,

0:28:44 > 0:28:47with colleagues.

0:28:47 > 0:28:49It could be which team we support.

0:28:51 > 0:28:53What style we go for.

0:28:54 > 0:28:57What our hobbies are.

0:28:57 > 0:29:00It gives comfort to belong to a group.

0:29:00 > 0:29:05And that gives us a critical clue into our success as a species.

0:29:07 > 0:29:10Survival of the fittest isn't just about individuals.

0:29:10 > 0:29:13It's also about groups.

0:29:13 > 0:29:17We're safer, we're more productive, we overcome challenges.

0:29:19 > 0:29:23The drive to work in groups has helped human populations

0:29:23 > 0:29:27thrive across the planet and build entire civilisations.

0:29:33 > 0:29:37And yet, there's a flipside to this drive to come together.

0:29:37 > 0:29:40Because for every ingroup, there are outsiders.

0:29:44 > 0:29:47And the consequences of that can be very dark.

0:29:52 > 0:29:56History is plagued with examples of one group turning on another

0:29:56 > 0:30:00that was defenceless and posed no threat.

0:30:03 > 0:30:06If you were to look at my family tree,

0:30:06 > 0:30:10you would see that most of the branches end in the early 1940s.

0:30:10 > 0:30:15This is because my family is ethnically Jewish.

0:30:15 > 0:30:19That small social marker was enough to prompt Nazi genocide.

0:30:23 > 0:30:25Under normal circumstances,

0:30:25 > 0:30:29you wouldn't find it conscionable to go and murder your neighbour.

0:30:29 > 0:30:33So what is it that allows hundreds or thousands of people to

0:30:33 > 0:30:36suddenly do exactly that?

0:30:36 > 0:30:41What is it about certain situations that short-circuits the normal

0:30:41 > 0:30:43social functioning of the brain?

0:30:47 > 0:30:52While the Nazi holocaust was on an unprecedented scale,

0:30:52 > 0:30:54it wasn't unique.

0:30:54 > 0:30:58Genocide continued to occur all over the world

0:30:58 > 0:31:02and within a generation, it returned to Eastern Europe.

0:31:02 > 0:31:05This time, it was in Yugoslavia.

0:31:15 > 0:31:22The Bosnian war from 1992 to '95 saw atrocities on both sides.

0:31:22 > 0:31:26In one of the worst, more than 100,000 Bosnians Muslims,

0:31:26 > 0:31:28known as Bosniaks,

0:31:28 > 0:31:32were slaughtered by Serbians in actions known as ethnic cleansing.

0:31:35 > 0:31:39One of the most horrible incidents happened here at Srebrenica.

0:31:43 > 0:31:46Over the course of just ten days,

0:31:46 > 0:31:498,000 people were systematically killed.

0:31:51 > 0:31:55How does something like this happen?

0:31:55 > 0:31:59Here in 1995, thousands of Bosniaks took refuge inside this

0:31:59 > 0:32:01United Nations compound

0:32:01 > 0:32:04because this village was surrounded by siege forces.

0:32:06 > 0:32:08But then, on July 11th,

0:32:08 > 0:32:12the UN commanders made the decision to expel all the refugees

0:32:12 > 0:32:16and they delivered them right into the hands of their enemies,

0:32:16 > 0:32:18who were waiting just outside this gate.

0:32:19 > 0:32:23Women were raped and men were executed

0:32:23 > 0:32:28and even children were killed and this was just the beginning of what

0:32:28 > 0:32:32would be the largest genocide on European soil since the Holocaust.

0:32:42 > 0:32:44The Dutch were there.

0:32:44 > 0:32:47I mean, the world was there, the UN,

0:32:47 > 0:32:50the Serbs were there as perpetrators.

0:32:50 > 0:32:52Everything was mixed.

0:32:52 > 0:32:55The refugees were there, the babies were crying.

0:32:56 > 0:32:59I was there, being protected with that UN ID card that said

0:32:59 > 0:33:02UN Language Assistant, whatever...

0:33:04 > 0:33:08Hasan Nuhanovic's status as a UN translator made him

0:33:08 > 0:33:10part of a protected group.

0:33:12 > 0:33:17But his family members were marked out by their identity as Muslims.

0:33:17 > 0:33:22At that very moment when my family was being sent out of the compound

0:33:22 > 0:33:26to actually die, I lost my mother, my brother and my father.

0:33:26 > 0:33:30You know, like, you are in a situation where your family

0:33:30 > 0:33:32is being killed.

0:33:32 > 0:33:35And I was thinking, "My God...

0:33:35 > 0:33:38"Why?"

0:33:40 > 0:33:42One of the most striking things

0:33:42 > 0:33:45is that the perpetrators weren't strangers.

0:33:45 > 0:33:48They were people with whom his family had previously shared

0:33:48 > 0:33:50a great deal.

0:33:51 > 0:33:56The continuation, you know, of the killings, of torture,

0:33:56 > 0:34:01was perpetrated by our neighbours.

0:34:01 > 0:34:05You know, the very people we had been living with for decades.

0:34:08 > 0:34:12They were capable of killing their own school friends.

0:34:14 > 0:34:18I remember they said they arrested a dentist who was a Bosniak,

0:34:18 > 0:34:20the best dentist in the town.

0:34:22 > 0:34:25They tied him up for a light pole, like this.

0:34:25 > 0:34:28In front of the post office. He was hanging there like this.

0:34:28 > 0:34:34And they beat him with a metal bar, they broke his spine.

0:34:34 > 0:34:39And he was there, dying for days, while Serb children went to school.

0:34:39 > 0:34:43Walking by his body, you know.

0:34:45 > 0:34:48I mean, there are universal values

0:34:48 > 0:34:53and these universal values are kind of very basic.

0:34:53 > 0:34:54Don't kill.

0:34:56 > 0:35:01April '92, this... Don't kill...

0:35:01 > 0:35:03suddenly disappeared.

0:35:03 > 0:35:05It was like - go and kill.

0:35:05 > 0:35:07It was allowed to kill.

0:35:18 > 0:35:22This is where Hasan's family is buried and each year,

0:35:22 > 0:35:25there are new bodies that are found and identified

0:35:25 > 0:35:26and they're brought here.

0:35:26 > 0:35:29Many of these graves are fresh.

0:35:32 > 0:35:35And across the human species, this is just one genocide of many.

0:35:37 > 0:35:42Genocides keep happening. Rwanda, Darfur, Nanking,

0:35:42 > 0:35:46Armenia... And my interest is in understanding why.

0:35:49 > 0:35:53Traditionally, we ask this question through the lens of history

0:35:53 > 0:35:57or economics or politics, and those are all important vantage points,

0:35:57 > 0:36:01but I think for a complete picture, one more lens is needed.

0:36:01 > 0:36:05We need to understand genocide as a neural phenomenon.

0:36:12 > 0:36:15I've been researching this back in my laboratory

0:36:15 > 0:36:20and here is my main question - when we interact with someone,

0:36:20 > 0:36:24does our brain function differ according to which group they're in?

0:36:27 > 0:36:28For every ingroup we belong to,

0:36:28 > 0:36:32there's at least one group that we don't.

0:36:32 > 0:36:35And that division can be based on anything.

0:36:37 > 0:36:41Race, or gender, or wealth, or religion.

0:36:47 > 0:36:50We put 130 participants in the scanner.

0:36:53 > 0:36:56And here's what they saw - six hands on the screen

0:36:56 > 0:36:58and the computer randomly picks one of these

0:36:58 > 0:37:01and then that hand gets stabbed by a syringe needle.

0:37:06 > 0:37:09Now, that activates the pain matrix, which is what comes online

0:37:09 > 0:37:12when you're in pain or you seem someone else in pain.

0:37:16 > 0:37:17Now, here's the trick.

0:37:17 > 0:37:20We now added a label to each hand -

0:37:20 > 0:37:25Jewish, Christian, Muslim, Hindu, Atheist, Scientologist.

0:37:25 > 0:37:28And the question is - would they care as much

0:37:28 > 0:37:31when they see a member of their outgroup getting stabbed?

0:37:40 > 0:37:42So, here's what we found.

0:37:42 > 0:37:47Here's a subject and when he watched a member of his ingroup getting

0:37:47 > 0:37:51stabbed, there was a large neural response in this area of his brain.

0:37:51 > 0:37:55But when he watched a member of one of his outgroups get stabbed,

0:37:55 > 0:38:00there was essentially a flat line.

0:38:00 > 0:38:02We scanned a range of volunteers

0:38:02 > 0:38:06and there are individual differences, but the trend is clear.

0:38:06 > 0:38:09A single-word label is enough

0:38:09 > 0:38:12to change your brain's basic preconscious response

0:38:12 > 0:38:14to another person in pain.

0:38:14 > 0:38:17In other words, how much you care about them.

0:38:17 > 0:38:20Now, you might have opinions about religion

0:38:20 > 0:38:22and its historical divisiveness,

0:38:22 > 0:38:27but even atheists here care more about other atheists'

0:38:27 > 0:38:30hands getting stabbed than they do about other people.

0:38:30 > 0:38:34So it's not really about religion. It's about which team you're on.

0:38:40 > 0:38:44This is just the first step in understanding how we get to this.

0:38:51 > 0:38:56To understand how groups of people can commit atrocities,

0:38:56 > 0:39:02it can help to look at the behaviour of individuals, like psychopaths.

0:39:02 > 0:39:03Some of the most callous,

0:39:03 > 0:39:09inhumane crimes ever recorded have been committed by psychopaths.

0:39:10 > 0:39:15But what's different about their brains that allows them

0:39:15 > 0:39:17to act that way?

0:39:19 > 0:39:23There are networks in the medial prefrontal cortex that

0:39:23 > 0:39:26underlie social interaction.

0:39:26 > 0:39:30When we interact with other people, this area becomes active.

0:39:32 > 0:39:36But in the brain of someone with extreme psychopathy, this area

0:39:36 > 0:39:39has a lot less activity.

0:39:40 > 0:39:43A psychopath doesn't care about you.

0:39:43 > 0:39:45He might be able to run a simulation

0:39:45 > 0:39:48of what you're going to do or how you might react,

0:39:48 > 0:39:52but when it comes to an emotional understanding

0:39:52 > 0:39:56of what it's like to be you, he doesn't get that.

0:39:56 > 0:40:00To him, you're just an obstacle to be worked around or manipulated,

0:40:00 > 0:40:03rather than a fellow human being.

0:40:03 > 0:40:09So what accounts for genocide? Is it driven by armies of psychopaths?

0:40:09 > 0:40:10Well, that can't be it

0:40:10 > 0:40:14because psychopaths only make up a small fraction of the population,

0:40:14 > 0:40:18but genocide typically engages a wider community.

0:40:18 > 0:40:23So here's the question - how do you get ordinary citizens on board?

0:40:29 > 0:40:33At the University of Leiden in Holland, Dr Lasana Harris

0:40:33 > 0:40:38has been conducting an experiment to understand a piece of this puzzle.

0:40:49 > 0:40:52So now we're going to start the experiment.

0:40:52 > 0:40:56What you're going to see is a bunch of pictures of different people.

0:40:56 > 0:40:59Your job is just to react naturally to those pictures.

0:41:01 > 0:41:03Lasana is looking at activity

0:41:03 > 0:41:07in the brain areas involved in human social interaction,

0:41:07 > 0:41:11in particular the medial prefrontal cortex.

0:41:11 > 0:41:14This comes online when we think about other people.

0:41:14 > 0:41:18It's less active when dealing with something inanimate, like a cup.

0:41:20 > 0:41:24What Lasana found is that this region has a similarly low

0:41:24 > 0:41:28response when we deal with certain types of other people.

0:41:32 > 0:41:34What he sees now are stereotypical

0:41:34 > 0:41:38images of people from different social groups.

0:41:41 > 0:41:44What we see here is that this network of brain regions,

0:41:44 > 0:41:48including medial prefrontal cortex, is less active

0:41:48 > 0:41:52when our participant looks at the homeless people.

0:41:52 > 0:41:54So what this pattern of activity

0:41:54 > 0:41:56suggests is a type of mental avoidance.

0:41:56 > 0:42:00They are not thinking about the mind of the homeless person

0:42:00 > 0:42:02in the same way they thought about the mind

0:42:02 > 0:42:05of the college student that they saw or the businesspeople.

0:42:08 > 0:42:09So if, for instance,

0:42:09 > 0:42:13you imagine that interacting with a homeless person will be unpleasant,

0:42:13 > 0:42:15it will make you feel bad.

0:42:15 > 0:42:18You may feel some demand to donate some of your money

0:42:18 > 0:42:22and all of these unpleasant pressures that come along with it.

0:42:22 > 0:42:26By shutting off the system, you never experience those feelings.

0:42:29 > 0:42:34To a brain that responds this way, homeless people are dehumanised.

0:42:34 > 0:42:39They are viewed more like objects and that can enable us to not care.

0:42:44 > 0:42:47Of course, if you don't properly diagnose this person

0:42:47 > 0:42:49as a human being - which is happening here -

0:42:49 > 0:42:52then the different moral rules we have

0:42:52 > 0:42:55that are reserved for human people may not apply.

0:42:59 > 0:43:01So, under the right circumstances,

0:43:01 > 0:43:05our brain activity can look more like a psychopath's.

0:43:06 > 0:43:10But to understand how we can get to genocide,

0:43:10 > 0:43:14we need to understand one more thing about group behaviour.

0:43:14 > 0:43:17Genocide is only possible

0:43:17 > 0:43:20when dehumanisation happens on a massive scale -

0:43:20 > 0:43:22not just a few individuals,

0:43:22 > 0:43:24but whole sections of the population.

0:43:26 > 0:43:30We are talking about a group of people committing atrocities.

0:43:33 > 0:43:37And if all the members of that perpetrating group are complicit,

0:43:37 > 0:43:40it's as if they have all somehow experienced

0:43:40 > 0:43:42the same reduction in brain activity

0:43:42 > 0:43:44when they think about their outgroup.

0:43:47 > 0:43:51This can be understood and studied like a disease outbreak -

0:43:51 > 0:43:56a kind of group contagion, one that is most often spread deliberately.

0:44:01 > 0:44:04The perfect tool for this job is propaganda.

0:44:04 > 0:44:06It plugs right into neural networks

0:44:06 > 0:44:11and it dials down the degree to which we care about other people.

0:44:11 > 0:44:14Just like all sites of genocide,

0:44:14 > 0:44:16that's what happened in the former Yugoslavia.

0:44:18 > 0:44:21The people who went on to torture and kill their neighbours

0:44:21 > 0:44:24were bombarded with propaganda.

0:44:24 > 0:44:29State-controlled broadcasters demonised the Bosnian Muslims

0:44:29 > 0:44:31with distorted news stories.

0:44:31 > 0:44:33We knew from the beginning

0:44:33 > 0:44:36that somebody is helping the Muslims and arming them.

0:44:36 > 0:44:39They went so far as to claim that the Muslims

0:44:39 > 0:44:42were feeding Serbian children to the lions at the zoo.

0:44:47 > 0:44:51Across place and time, the language of propaganda changes very little.

0:44:51 > 0:44:55It always plays the familiar tune of dehumanisation.

0:44:55 > 0:44:58"Make your enemy less than human.

0:44:58 > 0:45:00"Make him like an animal."

0:45:03 > 0:45:04Propaganda is a weapon.

0:45:08 > 0:45:10And, over the course of human history,

0:45:10 > 0:45:14it's become an art and a science

0:45:14 > 0:45:17and it's become ever more dangerous.

0:45:24 > 0:45:26In our connected age,

0:45:26 > 0:45:31any extremist group can reach millions of people with a keystroke.

0:45:31 > 0:45:35The internet is the perfect carrier for propaganda messages

0:45:35 > 0:45:38to reach the people most likely to act upon them -

0:45:38 > 0:45:40young men.

0:45:44 > 0:45:47The political agendas around us

0:45:47 > 0:45:51actually manipulate the brain activity inside of us.

0:45:52 > 0:45:55So is there any way to stop what has happened in the past

0:45:55 > 0:45:58from continuing into the future?

0:46:01 > 0:46:05One possible solution lies in an 1960s experiment

0:46:05 > 0:46:08that was conducted not in a science lab,

0:46:08 > 0:46:10but a school.

0:46:13 > 0:46:15It was 1968,

0:46:15 > 0:46:19the day after the assassination of Martin Luther King.

0:46:24 > 0:46:26Is there anyone in the United States

0:46:26 > 0:46:28that we do not treat as our brothers?

0:46:28 > 0:46:30- Yeah.- Who?- Black people.

0:46:30 > 0:46:32The black people. Who else?

0:46:32 > 0:46:35Jane Elliott was a teacher in a small town in Iowa

0:46:35 > 0:46:40and she wanted to show her class what prejudice really felt like.

0:46:40 > 0:46:41How are black people treated?

0:46:41 > 0:46:43They don't get anything in this world.

0:46:43 > 0:46:47- Why is that? - Because they are a different colour.

0:46:50 > 0:46:53These two men were in that class.

0:46:53 > 0:46:55This was Rex, back then...

0:46:58 > 0:46:59..and this was Ray.

0:47:01 > 0:47:04How many of you in here have blue eyes?

0:47:05 > 0:47:08OK. How many in here have brown eyes?

0:47:08 > 0:47:11Jane says, "We are going to have this exercise,"

0:47:11 > 0:47:14and she right away launches into the propaganda

0:47:14 > 0:47:16of "blue eyes are better than brown eyes".

0:47:16 > 0:47:20Blue-eyed people are better than brown-eyed people.

0:47:22 > 0:47:26Ray and Rex both had blue eyes.

0:47:27 > 0:47:30You brown-eyed people are not to play

0:47:30 > 0:47:32with the blue-eyed people on the playground

0:47:32 > 0:47:35because you are not as good as blue-eyed people.

0:47:35 > 0:47:39The brown-eyeds were denied privileges given to the blue eyes,

0:47:39 > 0:47:41and they had to wear special collars.

0:47:41 > 0:47:43You begin to notice today

0:47:43 > 0:47:46that we spend a great deal of time waiting for brown-eyed people.

0:47:54 > 0:47:58Do you remember what your own behaviour was like

0:47:58 > 0:48:00when you were on top?

0:48:00 > 0:48:05- I was...tremendously evil to my friends.- How so?

0:48:05 > 0:48:11I was going out of my way to pick on my brown-eyed friends

0:48:11 > 0:48:14for the sake of my own promotion.

0:48:14 > 0:48:16What did you do?

0:48:18 > 0:48:24I recall...telling Mrs Elliott - Jane - that she should keep

0:48:24 > 0:48:27the yardstick at hand in case those brown-eyeds got out of control.

0:48:27 > 0:48:29I don't see the yardstick, do you?

0:48:29 > 0:48:32It's over there.

0:48:32 > 0:48:34Mrs Elliott, you'd better keep that on your desk,

0:48:34 > 0:48:37so as the brown-eyed people don't get out of hand.

0:48:38 > 0:48:42At that time, my hair was quite blonde and my eyes were quite blue,

0:48:42 > 0:48:44and I was the perfect little Nazi.

0:48:45 > 0:48:50I looked for ways to be mean to my friends

0:48:50 > 0:48:56who, minutes or hours earlier, had been very close to me.

0:48:58 > 0:49:02But next day, there was a reversal of fortune.

0:49:04 > 0:49:05Yesterday, I told you

0:49:05 > 0:49:08that brown-eyed people aren't as good as blue-eyed people.

0:49:08 > 0:49:11That wasn't true. I lied to you, yesterday.

0:49:13 > 0:49:15Oh, boy, here we go again.

0:49:15 > 0:49:16The truth is...

0:49:17 > 0:49:20..that brown-eyed people are better than blue-eyed people.

0:49:20 > 0:49:22The person you trust stands before you

0:49:22 > 0:49:27and says, "I was wrong. Now, here's the truth."

0:49:28 > 0:49:32Takes your world and shatters it,

0:49:32 > 0:49:35like you have never had your world shattered before.

0:49:35 > 0:49:39You blue-eyed people are not to play with the brown-eyed people.

0:49:39 > 0:49:41Blue-eyed people, go to the back,

0:49:41 > 0:49:43the brown-eyed people, come to the front.

0:49:46 > 0:49:47It's not fair!

0:49:50 > 0:49:52Tell me a little more about what it was like

0:49:52 > 0:49:54when you were in the down group.

0:49:54 > 0:49:59You have such a sense of loss of personality and self,

0:49:59 > 0:50:01that makes it almost impossible to function

0:50:01 > 0:50:03with what is going on in the room.

0:50:03 > 0:50:06Should the colour of some other person's eyes

0:50:06 > 0:50:08have anything to do you with how you treat them?

0:50:08 > 0:50:10- CLASS: No. - All right, then -

0:50:10 > 0:50:11should the colour of their skin?

0:50:11 > 0:50:13CLASS: No.

0:50:13 > 0:50:17Should you judge people by the colour of their skin?

0:50:17 > 0:50:18CLASS: No.

0:50:19 > 0:50:21If I were just going to riff, guess at it,

0:50:21 > 0:50:24it's that one of the most important things we learn as humans

0:50:24 > 0:50:25is perspective-taking

0:50:25 > 0:50:30and kids don't often get really meaningful exercise in that,

0:50:30 > 0:50:32and when you're forced into understanding

0:50:32 > 0:50:34what it is like to stand in someone else's shoes,

0:50:34 > 0:50:37that opens up a lot of cognitive pathways for you.

0:50:37 > 0:50:41I remember saying something to my dad about a comment he made,

0:50:41 > 0:50:45saying "No, that's not appropriate."

0:50:45 > 0:50:48And it did change within the family.

0:50:48 > 0:50:53But you talk about a little kid making that statement, it's huge.

0:50:53 > 0:50:56But it reaffirmed that you can do that -

0:50:56 > 0:50:58you could begin to change.

0:51:06 > 0:51:09The brilliance of the blue eyes/brown eyes experiment

0:51:09 > 0:51:13is that the teacher, Jane Elliott, switched which group was on top

0:51:13 > 0:51:17and that allowed the students to extract the larger lesson,

0:51:17 > 0:51:20which is that systems of rules can be arbitrary.

0:51:20 > 0:51:25They learned that the truths of the world are not fixed

0:51:25 > 0:51:27and they are not even necessarily truths.

0:51:27 > 0:51:30And this is what empowered the children as they grew

0:51:30 > 0:51:34to see through the smoke and mirrors of other people's political agendas

0:51:34 > 0:51:36and to form their own opinions -

0:51:36 > 0:51:40surely a skill we should be teaching to all of our children.

0:51:41 > 0:51:43Should the colour of some other person's eyes

0:51:43 > 0:51:45have anything to do with how you treat them?

0:51:45 > 0:51:46CLASS: No.

0:51:47 > 0:51:50GUNFIRE

0:51:50 > 0:51:52When people are armed with an understanding

0:51:52 > 0:51:54of how propaganda works,

0:51:54 > 0:51:58the power of propaganda is reduced.

0:51:59 > 0:52:03As we come to understand the deep importance of cooperation,

0:52:03 > 0:52:08we stand a chance of not only reducing dehumanisation,

0:52:08 > 0:52:11but achieving our potential as a species.

0:52:14 > 0:52:17Genocide doesn't have to be the norm.

0:52:25 > 0:52:28Instead, our fundamentally social nature

0:52:28 > 0:52:32can hold the key to our success as a species.

0:52:34 > 0:52:37Our future, our survival,

0:52:37 > 0:52:42is intimately, permanently bound up with that of the people around us.

0:52:45 > 0:52:47Our social drive is at the root

0:52:47 > 0:52:53of extraordinary acts of bravery and generosity.

0:52:55 > 0:52:59Who you are has everything to do with who we are.

0:52:59 > 0:53:02Our brains are so fundamentally wired to interact

0:53:02 > 0:53:06that it is not always clear where each of us begins and ends.

0:53:11 > 0:53:16Our species is more than just seven billion individuals,

0:53:16 > 0:53:17spread out across the planet.

0:53:19 > 0:53:23We are something more like a single, vast super-organism.

0:53:25 > 0:53:29Because what your friends know and love as you

0:53:29 > 0:53:31is really a neural network,

0:53:31 > 0:53:36embedded in a far larger web of other neural networks.

0:53:40 > 0:53:42In this age of digital connection,

0:53:42 > 0:53:46we desperately need to understand the links between humans.

0:53:46 > 0:53:50If we want our civilisations to have a bright future,

0:53:50 > 0:53:53we'll need to understand how human brains interact,

0:53:53 > 0:53:55the dangers and the opportunities,

0:53:55 > 0:53:57because there is no avoiding the truth

0:53:57 > 0:54:00that is etched into our neural circuitry.

0:54:00 > 0:54:02We need each other.

0:54:08 > 0:54:11Next time on The Brain, I am going to look into the future.

0:54:13 > 0:54:18What if the brain could do more? Handle more?

0:54:18 > 0:54:21What if there were other ways for it to operate?

0:54:23 > 0:54:26We will look at how we can use our brains

0:54:26 > 0:54:28to control new kinds of bodies.

0:54:33 > 0:54:38How our sensory experience can be expanded to new horizons.

0:54:40 > 0:54:43We will look at how we might one day separate our minds

0:54:43 > 0:54:44from our physical selves.

0:54:46 > 0:54:49What if the study of the brain could address our mortality?

0:54:49 > 0:54:53What if, in the future, we didn't have to die?

0:54:55 > 0:54:57So, what is next for our brains?

0:54:57 > 0:55:01What do the next thousand years have in store for us?

0:55:01 > 0:55:05And, in the far future, what is the human race going to look like?

0:55:05 > 0:55:07What will we be capable of?

0:55:08 > 0:55:12We are heading for a fundamental change in the relationship

0:55:12 > 0:55:16between the body, the brain and the outside world.

0:55:17 > 0:55:20We are marrying our biology with our technology

0:55:20 > 0:55:25and that is poised to transform who we will be.