What's in Your Head?

Download Subtitles

Transcript

0:00:02 > 0:00:03Meet Charlie and Iona.

0:00:03 > 0:00:06As you can plainly see, Charlie is much taller than Iona.

0:00:06 > 0:00:10But sometimes reality is not what it seems.

0:00:55 > 0:00:59Welcome to the 2011 Royal Institution Christmas Lectures,

0:00:59 > 0:01:01Meet Your Brain.

0:01:01 > 0:01:02Let me introduce myself.

0:01:02 > 0:01:06I'm Bruce Hood, and I am a scientist interested in the human brain,

0:01:06 > 0:01:10what it is and how it changes as we grow older.

0:01:10 > 0:01:15Actually, when I said "I am Bruce Hood" what I really should have said is, "This is Bruce Hood".

0:01:15 > 0:01:19Because everything I am is really a product of my brain.

0:01:19 > 0:01:21It's not my heart, it's not my kidneys -

0:01:21 > 0:01:24these are important organs, but I could have them transplanted

0:01:24 > 0:01:26and I'd still be the same person.

0:01:26 > 0:01:30That's because it's our brain that makes us who we are.

0:01:30 > 0:01:33In this series of lectures, we're going to be looking at the human brain,

0:01:33 > 0:01:38what it is, how it works and most importantly, how it interacts with other brains.

0:01:38 > 0:01:42But before I do so, I'd like to introduce you to someone else here.

0:01:42 > 0:01:47Or rather, someone who is here no longer.

0:01:48 > 0:01:52This is a real human brain, from a person.

0:01:54 > 0:01:58Before they died, they made the decision to donate their brain to science,

0:01:58 > 0:02:00so that we could discover

0:02:00 > 0:02:05the workings of this most astonishing, amazing organ.

0:02:08 > 0:02:10It is so mysterious and complicated,

0:02:10 > 0:02:13we can't even begin to really know how it works!

0:02:15 > 0:02:18I brought this brain along tonight to remind you

0:02:18 > 0:02:22exactly who we are, and what we are trying to understand.

0:02:23 > 0:02:26It is truly awe-inspiring.

0:02:29 > 0:02:32Every one of you here tonight, and watching at home,

0:02:32 > 0:02:34has a brain inside your head.

0:02:34 > 0:02:38Every brain is important. Every brain is unique.

0:02:38 > 0:02:41But all brains have the same basic machinery -

0:02:41 > 0:02:44so how does a brain work?

0:02:44 > 0:02:47To help me answer these questions,

0:02:47 > 0:02:51I've invited along another brain scientist, Professor Vince Walsh from UCL.

0:02:51 > 0:02:53Give a big round of applause to Vince!

0:02:53 > 0:02:55APPLAUSE AND CHEERING

0:02:59 > 0:03:02So Vince, you've brought along this special machine

0:03:02 > 0:03:05that influences the brain.

0:03:05 > 0:03:06We're going to see it in action.

0:03:06 > 0:03:09But before we do, let's start with a simple demonstration.

0:03:09 > 0:03:13Do you know the Nursery Rhyme "Baa Baa Black Sheep"?

0:03:13 > 0:03:15- I think so.- Could you give us the first line?

0:03:15 > 0:03:18Baa baa black sheep, have you any wool?

0:03:18 > 0:03:19That's good.

0:03:19 > 0:03:23Reassuring to know that professors still know their nursery rhymes.

0:03:23 > 0:03:26OK. I'm going to get you to repeat that,

0:03:26 > 0:03:30but this time, I'm going to stimulate this part of your brain

0:03:30 > 0:03:31with your machine.

0:03:31 > 0:03:33Start, please.

0:03:33 > 0:03:34Baa baa black...

0:03:34 > 0:03:36VOICE BECOMES DISTORTED

0:03:36 > 0:03:39LAUGHTER

0:03:40 > 0:03:41Don't be alarmed.

0:03:42 > 0:03:46- You're perfectly OK, Vince, aren't you?- I'm fine, yes. - That's very good.

0:03:46 > 0:03:49Somehow this machine has disrupted Vince's brain.

0:03:49 > 0:03:52This is a trans-magnetic stimulator.

0:03:52 > 0:03:54It's delivering a very powerful magnetic pulse

0:03:54 > 0:03:56for a brief fraction of a second

0:03:56 > 0:04:00but why is it disrupting Vince's ability to speak?

0:04:00 > 0:04:02I'm going to give you a clue with the next demonstration.

0:04:02 > 0:04:05Have a look at this old television over here.

0:04:05 > 0:04:07I'm going to need a magnet.

0:04:08 > 0:04:11You can see the image on the television is perfectly normal

0:04:11 > 0:04:14but look what happens when I bring a magnet close to it.

0:04:18 > 0:04:22You can see the image is being distorted by the magnet.

0:04:22 > 0:04:24Now, why is this?

0:04:24 > 0:04:27Well, remarkably, the answer comes from none other

0:04:27 > 0:04:30than the founder of the Royal Institution Christmas Lectures,

0:04:30 > 0:04:33Michael Faraday, because Faraday discovered over 150 years ago

0:04:33 > 0:04:36and often demonstrated in this very theatre

0:04:36 > 0:04:41that there's a relationship between magnets and electrical fields.

0:04:41 > 0:04:45The image on the television is produced by an electrified beam

0:04:45 > 0:04:47so that when I bring a magnet close to it,

0:04:47 > 0:04:49it's distorting the path of that beam

0:04:49 > 0:04:52and that's why the image is being distorted.

0:04:52 > 0:04:55So let's put these pieces of information together.

0:04:55 > 0:04:57A magnet can disrupt an electrical field.

0:04:57 > 0:05:00We know the magnet is disrupting Vince's ability to speak

0:05:00 > 0:05:05which is a product of his brain so we can conclude that the brain

0:05:05 > 0:05:09must be using some form of electrical communication to make you speak.

0:05:09 > 0:05:12- Is that roughly correct, Vince? - True, yeah.- OK, Vince.

0:05:12 > 0:05:14If I put a magnet next to my head,

0:05:14 > 0:05:19it doesn't seem to disrupt any of my ability to speak, so why is that?

0:05:19 > 0:05:23That's because the field you've got next to your head is static.

0:05:23 > 0:05:26Even though you're moving at about, it's moving very slowly.

0:05:26 > 0:05:31To create an electric field that disturbs brain cells, it's got to move very, very rapidly.

0:05:31 > 0:05:35Every one of those clicks you hear is 1/10000 of a millisecond

0:05:35 > 0:05:38so the field has to change very, very quickly

0:05:38 > 0:05:41to create any electricity in the brain.

0:05:41 > 0:05:44Would you like me to do some more disruption of Vince's brain?

0:05:44 > 0:05:46- AUDIENCE:- Yes. - Vince, I'm sorry,

0:05:46 > 0:05:51but we're going to put you through some more. OK, so, what else shall we try?

0:05:51 > 0:05:53How about some motor control?

0:05:53 > 0:05:56We could do that. We could try left and right-handed.

0:05:56 > 0:06:00Can we try just affecting your right hand, would that be OK?

0:06:00 > 0:06:03Right, so I'm going to take my magnet. Is it fully charged up again?

0:06:03 > 0:06:07- It is.- I'll touch my nose a few times.

0:06:07 > 0:06:10Let's see how accurate it you are when I place it up here.

0:06:10 > 0:06:12- Is that the correct area?- Mhm.

0:06:12 > 0:06:15ELECTRICAL CLICKING

0:06:15 > 0:06:16AUDIENCE LAUGHS

0:06:16 > 0:06:20Vince, tell the audience what that feels like?

0:06:20 > 0:06:24It's actually quite painless but I've lost control of my muscles.

0:06:24 > 0:06:30I've lost control of my ability to locate my hand in space.

0:06:30 > 0:06:33- It's a very, very weird sensation. - Can we do one last example?

0:06:33 > 0:06:35Can you, um...can you clap for us?

0:06:35 > 0:06:39I'm just going to stick the magnet here, here we go.

0:06:39 > 0:06:41You ready? Start clapping, please.

0:06:42 > 0:06:43ELECTRICAL CLICKING

0:06:43 > 0:06:44AUDIENCE LAUGHS

0:06:44 > 0:06:47OK, I think we've put you through enough, Vince.

0:06:47 > 0:06:50Before you go, tell us, please.

0:06:50 > 0:06:53We've seen how a magnet can disrupt normal function,

0:06:53 > 0:06:54but does it have any application?

0:06:54 > 0:06:57It does. What we've done has been very dramatic.

0:06:57 > 0:07:00We've been using very high magnetic fields to influence the brain

0:07:00 > 0:07:03so we can see that it does affect the brain but if we use lower fields

0:07:03 > 0:07:07and stimulate specific areas of the brain, we can work on treating

0:07:07 > 0:07:11things like depression, or modelling brain disorders

0:07:11 > 0:07:13or modelling stroke in real research,

0:07:13 > 0:07:15so it's got lots and lots of applications.

0:07:15 > 0:07:17Vince, that's been absolutely fascinating.

0:07:17 > 0:07:20Would everyone give Vince a round of applause.

0:07:20 > 0:07:22APPLAUSE

0:07:32 > 0:07:35Do you realise we've just proved that the brain

0:07:35 > 0:07:38is an electrical system, and I think that's pretty cool.

0:07:38 > 0:07:41Not only do magnets disrupt brain function

0:07:41 > 0:07:44but we can use magnets to look at the workings of the brain.

0:07:44 > 0:07:47But to do so you have to have a very powerful magnet.

0:07:47 > 0:07:50And we happen to have access to one not here in London

0:07:50 > 0:07:54but up in Cheltenham where we've set up a live link.

0:07:54 > 0:07:56We recorded this earlier.

0:07:56 > 0:07:57Hello, I'm Dr Thalia Gjersoe

0:07:57 > 0:08:01and I'm here at the MRI scanner so you can read my mind.

0:08:01 > 0:08:06I'm here with Iain Lyburn who's going to take us through it.

0:08:06 > 0:08:08Welcome to the Cheltenham Imaging Centre.

0:08:08 > 0:08:11It's run by the Cobalt Appeal Fund

0:08:11 > 0:08:14and houses imaging for PET and MRI

0:08:14 > 0:08:18and today we're going to have an MRI scan looking at your brain, seeing how it works.

0:08:18 > 0:08:21What I'd like to do is show you the scanner first of all.

0:08:21 > 0:08:25- Have you seen it before? - No, I haven't. - Shall we've a look inside?- OK.

0:08:28 > 0:08:32It's got a big metal door because it's housed in a big metal cage.

0:08:32 > 0:08:33This is the scanner.

0:08:33 > 0:08:36You'll be going in with your head in the scanner

0:08:36 > 0:08:40and it's got a very strong magnetic field which is part of the way it works.

0:08:40 > 0:08:43Karen's going to show us how strong the magnet is.

0:08:43 > 0:08:47- She's actually got...- I've got a spanner on the end of a bit of rope.

0:08:55 > 0:08:59You can see how powerful it is. So that's how strong it is.

0:09:00 > 0:09:02Quite impressive that, actually, isn't it?

0:09:02 > 0:09:06- Wow. Iain, can you hear me? - Yes, hi, good evening.

0:09:06 > 0:09:09We've just seen this magnet of yours.

0:09:09 > 0:09:12It's extraordinarily powerful. How powerful is it?

0:09:12 > 0:09:1530,000 times as strong as the magnetic field of the Earth.

0:09:15 > 0:09:19That sounds dangerous to me. Why doesn't it affect the human brain

0:09:19 > 0:09:21when you put someone in a scanner?

0:09:21 > 0:09:23Well, the magnet's actually fixed.

0:09:23 > 0:09:25It doesn't move, there's no movement,

0:09:25 > 0:09:28so it's safe to go in and use for imaging.

0:09:29 > 0:09:33So it's not like the TMS. It's a static magnetic field.

0:09:33 > 0:09:37- Is that correct?- It's a static magnetic field, so it's safe.- OK.

0:09:37 > 0:09:40- Thalia, can you hear me? - Yes, I can, Bruce. Hello.

0:09:40 > 0:09:43I wouldn't go into that machine with anything magnetic.

0:09:43 > 0:09:46Have you got something small that you can put in your hand?

0:09:46 > 0:09:50- I do. I've got a walnut. - You've got a walnut? All right.

0:09:50 > 0:09:54Well, I want you to take the walnut into the scanner, OK,

0:09:54 > 0:09:56and don't tell us which hand you're going to put it in

0:09:56 > 0:10:00because were we're going to do a bit of mind reading, I think.

0:10:00 > 0:10:04We'll be seeing a bit more of you later on. Or, should I say, we'll be seeing more of your brain.

0:10:04 > 0:10:06For the moment, though,

0:10:06 > 0:10:10can we give a warm round of applause to Thalia and Professor Iain Lyburn.

0:10:10 > 0:10:14APPLAUSE

0:10:16 > 0:10:19So all brains work by electrical signalling

0:10:19 > 0:10:23and all brains are very similar but they can also be quite different, in some ways.

0:10:23 > 0:10:26Take a look at all of these animal brains.

0:10:26 > 0:10:29Take a guess at which brain you think this animal belongs to.

0:10:29 > 0:10:32I'm going to give you a pound coin so you can estimate the size.

0:10:32 > 0:10:36- Now, what animal do you think that might be? Shout it out.- Spider!

0:10:36 > 0:10:37A spider?

0:10:37 > 0:10:40- No, it's not a spider. Can we have another example?- Mouse!

0:10:40 > 0:10:45Mouse? Who said mouse? Hands up. You're correct. It's a mouse.

0:10:45 > 0:10:46OK, what about this creature here?

0:10:46 > 0:10:47Rat.

0:10:47 > 0:10:49Did someone say rat?

0:10:49 > 0:10:53AUDIENCE CALL OUT

0:10:53 > 0:10:56It's a rat's brain. What about this brain here?

0:10:56 > 0:11:00AUDIENCE CALL OUT

0:11:00 > 0:11:04It's a...it's a chicken. Believe it or not, that's a chicken's brain.

0:11:04 > 0:11:06And what about this one here?

0:11:06 > 0:11:08AUDIENCE CALL OUT

0:11:08 > 0:11:10A tortoise? No. No.

0:11:10 > 0:11:11AUDIENCE CALL OUT

0:11:11 > 0:11:16It's a cat's brain. And here we have... a dog.

0:11:17 > 0:11:20And finally this one. What's this one belong to?

0:11:20 > 0:11:21AUDIENCE CALL OUT

0:11:21 > 0:11:23A horse. Who said a horse?

0:11:23 > 0:11:25Hands up if you said horse.

0:11:25 > 0:11:27Let's consider the horse for a moment.

0:11:27 > 0:11:31It's a very large animal, isn't it? A horse is on average three times bigger than a human

0:11:31 > 0:11:35but look at the horse's brain in comparison to the human brain.

0:11:35 > 0:11:37Let me take it around to show you.

0:11:38 > 0:11:40Even though a horse is much larger than a human,

0:11:40 > 0:11:43the brain is actually smaller

0:11:43 > 0:11:45so this shows you the size of the body size

0:11:45 > 0:11:47doesn't predict the size of the brain.

0:11:47 > 0:11:51In fact, if you think about the size of a human body on average,

0:11:51 > 0:11:55our brain is seven times larger than you would ever imagine.

0:11:55 > 0:11:59So, who would like to hold a human brain?

0:11:59 > 0:12:01That's an awful lot of you.

0:12:01 > 0:12:03Unfortunately, we can't let you hold the human brain

0:12:03 > 0:12:06but I just happen to have one which is just as good,

0:12:06 > 0:12:08made out of synthetic plastic over here.

0:12:12 > 0:12:13It's a very good copy.

0:12:13 > 0:12:18It's the same shape, it's the same size and it's the same weight.

0:12:18 > 0:12:20So, would you like to hold the human brain?

0:12:21 > 0:12:24Who wants to halt the human brain. You do? Put your hands out.

0:12:26 > 0:12:29- What's the first thing you notice about it?- It's quite heavy.

0:12:29 > 0:12:33It extraordinarily heavy, isn't it? It's about one half pounds.

0:12:33 > 0:12:34You can pass it along.

0:12:34 > 0:12:36One and a half kilos, I should say. OK.

0:12:38 > 0:12:40It's really quite heavy.

0:12:40 > 0:12:42What else is it? What else do you notice about it?

0:12:42 > 0:12:44Let me tell you.

0:12:44 > 0:12:46Can I have my brain back? Thank you.

0:12:47 > 0:12:49The brain, as you can see,

0:12:49 > 0:12:53is made up of two halves and each half is called a hemisphere.

0:12:53 > 0:12:56The interesting thing about the hemisphere is it controls

0:12:56 > 0:12:59the opposite side of the body, so if you remember

0:12:59 > 0:13:01when we were stimulating Vince's brain

0:13:01 > 0:13:03with the trans-magnetic stimulator,

0:13:03 > 0:13:06when I was activating his left side of the brain,

0:13:06 > 0:13:10it was his right hand which was being affected.

0:13:10 > 0:13:13Now, we don't really know why the brain is organised like that.

0:13:13 > 0:13:15It's a little bit of a mystery.

0:13:15 > 0:13:17In fact, you probably wouldn't be aware

0:13:17 > 0:13:22unless you've got some damage on the opposite side and you noticed the behaviour was affected.

0:13:22 > 0:13:26The other thing about the brain which I think is quite fascinating

0:13:26 > 0:13:27is all these folds and creases

0:13:27 > 0:13:30because all brains have this to some extent

0:13:30 > 0:13:34but if you look at the example of the mouse brain or the rat brain,

0:13:34 > 0:13:36they're really quite smooth.

0:13:36 > 0:13:40In the human, these folds and creases are quite pronounced

0:13:40 > 0:13:41so why is that?

0:13:41 > 0:13:44To get an answer for that, you really have to zoom in

0:13:44 > 0:13:46to the building blocks of the brain

0:13:46 > 0:13:49to a special kind of cell called a neuron and here we have

0:13:49 > 0:13:52the image of a neuron and as you can see

0:13:52 > 0:13:55it looks like a kind of strange alien creature from outer space

0:13:55 > 0:13:57and there's a lot of them.

0:13:57 > 0:14:00There are about 100 billion neurons in the average brain.

0:14:00 > 0:14:03And all these tentacles are dendrites

0:14:03 > 0:14:06and this is the way neurons are communicating with each other,

0:14:06 > 0:14:09by sending electrical impulses.

0:14:09 > 0:14:12Each neuron typically has one very thick connecting fibre

0:14:12 > 0:14:15called the axon and it's the axon that sends out information

0:14:15 > 0:14:18to connect up with all the others.

0:14:18 > 0:14:21Now, it turns out that the neurons

0:14:21 > 0:14:24and the connections which are related to those things we consider

0:14:24 > 0:14:28intelligence and being clever, they're not throughout the brain.

0:14:28 > 0:14:31They're concentrated in just the outer layer of the brain,

0:14:31 > 0:14:35a layer that's only 3-4 millimetres thick.

0:14:35 > 0:14:36We call this the cortex.

0:14:36 > 0:14:40The cortex comes from the Latin word for bark.

0:14:40 > 0:14:44So it's this outer layer with all these connections which make us

0:14:44 > 0:14:47very clever and flexible in our thinking.

0:14:47 > 0:14:49So it's not so much the size of the brain that's important.

0:14:49 > 0:14:52Rather, it's the surface area of the cortex

0:14:52 > 0:14:56and how big that is that allows for all these connections.

0:14:56 > 0:14:59If you take the human brain and flatten it out,

0:14:59 > 0:15:01it would have this degree of surface area.

0:15:01 > 0:15:03Would you hold my brain for a moment?

0:15:03 > 0:15:07OK, so this is how big the human cortex is if you flatten it out.

0:15:07 > 0:15:13That's a very big area, so how do you get all that inside a normal head?

0:15:13 > 0:15:15Well, here's nature's solution.

0:15:16 > 0:15:17It's all folded up.

0:15:17 > 0:15:20So nature has come up with an answer

0:15:20 > 0:15:23for, basically, an engineering problem.

0:15:23 > 0:15:24Thank you very much.

0:15:26 > 0:15:30Now, if it wasn't all folded up like that,

0:15:30 > 0:15:33then your head would have to be half as big again, which is not a good look

0:15:33 > 0:15:36and, for any mothers watching, it's bad enough giving birth

0:15:36 > 0:15:40to a baby of a normal sized head without it being any larger.

0:15:40 > 0:15:45OK, so let's now consider some other animals. Look at these little guys.

0:15:47 > 0:15:49They're quite exotic, aren't they?

0:15:49 > 0:15:53Does anyone know what they are? They're jellyfish, that's right.

0:15:53 > 0:15:55They're Australian Blubber Jellyfish

0:15:55 > 0:15:59and you can see them moving around in the pool.

0:15:59 > 0:16:02So what's so special about the brain of a jellyfish?

0:16:02 > 0:16:03Would anyone like to answer?

0:16:03 > 0:16:06- Sir?- Is it transparent? - That's a good answer.

0:16:06 > 0:16:10- Anything else? Yes? - It doesn't have one?

0:16:10 > 0:16:12It doesn't have one. Well done.

0:16:12 > 0:16:15A trick question. They do have a central nervous system

0:16:15 > 0:16:18but they don't really have a brain as such.

0:16:18 > 0:16:20So if jellyfish don't have brains,

0:16:20 > 0:16:23then why do all these other animals have brains?

0:16:23 > 0:16:26Why do you think we have a brain in the first place?

0:16:26 > 0:16:28Who would like to answer? Yes.

0:16:28 > 0:16:30Because otherwise we wouldn't be alive?

0:16:30 > 0:16:33Otherwise we wouldn't be alive, that's good. Any more answers?

0:16:33 > 0:16:35- Yes?- Memory.

0:16:35 > 0:16:38These are all great answers but the basic answer for all the animals

0:16:38 > 0:16:43which have brains is that we use brains to navigate around the world.

0:16:43 > 0:16:44The jellyfish can move

0:16:44 > 0:16:47but it's not keeping track of where it's going and jellyfish

0:16:47 > 0:16:51tend to go with the ebb and flow of the tides whereas animals that have

0:16:51 > 0:16:55brains are using them to navigate their world, to find food,

0:16:55 > 0:16:56to seek mates, to avoid predators

0:16:56 > 0:16:59and to keep track of where they're going

0:16:59 > 0:17:01in order to plan their movements in the world.

0:17:01 > 0:17:05So brains are for figuring out and predicting what's going to go next.

0:17:05 > 0:17:08If you think about it, an animal, or an animal like us,

0:17:08 > 0:17:13is really a kind of complex mobile moving factory made up of many subdivisions,

0:17:13 > 0:17:16different processing plants, recycling centres

0:17:16 > 0:17:19and movement machinery, and that all has to be coordinated.

0:17:19 > 0:17:22If it wasn't, we'd fall apart

0:17:22 > 0:17:26so brains are really for controlling all these different activities.

0:17:26 > 0:17:29Now some of these activities are fairly automated.

0:17:29 > 0:17:32For example, breathing and hearts are controlled by the brainstem

0:17:32 > 0:17:36which is below the cortex, so it doesn't require a lot of consciousness.

0:17:36 > 0:17:41And other things like your movements that you've learnt well, you don't have to think about them.

0:17:41 > 0:17:46Even walking. You know where you're going and you can plan that, but coordinating the movements,

0:17:46 > 0:17:48you don't have to think about that.

0:17:48 > 0:17:51And that's controlled by the cerebellum at the bottom here.

0:17:51 > 0:17:53So whether they're automatic or controlled,

0:17:53 > 0:17:56the whole point is they still need to be coordinated by a system

0:17:56 > 0:17:59and that's what the cortex does, sitting up here.

0:17:59 > 0:18:01So the information is flooding up into the brain

0:18:01 > 0:18:03through the central nervous system.

0:18:03 > 0:18:07The information from the most extreme parts, for example the arms

0:18:07 > 0:18:12or even the legs, they form part of the peripheral nervous system.

0:18:12 > 0:18:14So how fast does a nerve impulse travel?

0:18:14 > 0:18:18Well, we're going to try a little experiment. We're going to measure the speed of a nerve impulse

0:18:18 > 0:18:22travelling the length of one arm and I'm going to need some volunteers.

0:18:22 > 0:18:27In fact, I'm going to need all of Row E, so stand up, row E.

0:18:27 > 0:18:29Big round of applause.

0:18:29 > 0:18:32APPLAUSE

0:18:36 > 0:18:39- What's your name, sir? - My name's Omar.- Omar?- Yep.

0:18:39 > 0:18:43- OK, Omar, and who do we have at the end?- Tim.- Hello, Tim.

0:18:43 > 0:18:47Omar, what I'm going to do is I'm going to grip your left shoulder with my right hand

0:18:47 > 0:18:50and with your right hand, you grip the left shoulder

0:18:50 > 0:18:52and everyone copy everyone so you form a chain reaction.

0:18:52 > 0:18:55OK. Are you all holding? Right.

0:18:55 > 0:19:00What I'm going to do is I'm going to squeeze your shoulder gently and everyone do it gently.

0:19:00 > 0:19:03When you feel your shoulder being squeezed, and not before,

0:19:03 > 0:19:07you squeeze your neighbour's shoulder, so it's going to pass the entire length of Row E.

0:19:07 > 0:19:10OK? And Tim at the end, when you feel your shoulder being squeezed,

0:19:10 > 0:19:14you shout stop because we're going to measure the speed,

0:19:14 > 0:19:18the time it takes for that response to travel the full length of Row E.

0:19:18 > 0:19:19You got it?

0:19:19 > 0:19:22So let's go.

0:19:24 > 0:19:28- Stop.- OK, that's pretty good. That's 3.10 seconds.

0:19:28 > 0:19:32Let's see if you can get a bit faster. All right, ready again?

0:19:36 > 0:19:41- Stop.- OK, that's just under three seconds. That's pretty good. You are getting better with practise.

0:19:41 > 0:19:44Now, this time I don't want you to grip the shoulder.

0:19:44 > 0:19:48I want you to hold hands. You should be even faster now, shouldn't you?

0:19:48 > 0:19:51Squeeze your neighbour's hand when you feel your left hand is squeezed

0:19:51 > 0:19:54and, again, Tim, you shout stop when you feel that.

0:19:54 > 0:19:55OK, ready?

0:19:59 > 0:20:00Stop.

0:20:00 > 0:20:05That's three seconds, almost a full second longer.

0:20:05 > 0:20:09Now, why does it take longer for the nerve impulse to travel the full distance?

0:20:09 > 0:20:11If you think about it, the first time we did it

0:20:11 > 0:20:14it's only travelling the length of one arm.

0:20:14 > 0:20:15But when we're holding hands,

0:20:15 > 0:20:20it has to travel the length of one arm plus an extra arm to the person next to you.

0:20:20 > 0:20:23An arm is almost a metre long, isn't it?

0:20:23 > 0:20:27And with 15 arms, that's an extra 15 metres it has to travel

0:20:27 > 0:20:31in just under about a second, so it's somewhere between 10 and 15 metres per second,

0:20:31 > 0:20:33which is about right for that kind of nerve impulse.

0:20:33 > 0:20:35So, a big round of applause for row E.

0:20:35 > 0:20:39APPLAUSE

0:20:41 > 0:20:44So, we were estimating the speed of a nerve impulse in the arm,

0:20:44 > 0:20:47and it's usually roundabout that kind of speed,

0:20:47 > 0:20:50and that's usually a lot slower than people imagine,

0:20:50 > 0:20:53because when you think about nerve impulses we often assume

0:20:53 > 0:20:56they must be almost as fast as electricity,

0:20:56 > 0:20:58because it seems like it is an electrical impulse.

0:20:58 > 0:21:04But, in fact, electricity travels about three million times faster than a nerve impulse.

0:21:04 > 0:21:07So, we've been using these sorts of experiments to try

0:21:07 > 0:21:10and estimate how the brain is working,

0:21:10 > 0:21:13but can you ever really measure directly nerve activity?

0:21:13 > 0:21:17Remarkably, you can if you're an expert and you know what you're doing,

0:21:17 > 0:21:19and you happen to have a very thin wire.

0:21:19 > 0:21:22So, would you give a warm welcome to two experts from Newcastle,

0:21:22 > 0:21:25Dr Claire Rind and Dr Peter Simmons.

0:21:25 > 0:21:29APPLAUSE

0:21:35 > 0:21:39So, Claire, I believe you've had some interesting travelling

0:21:39 > 0:21:41companions with you, is that right?

0:21:41 > 0:21:44We have, we've come down on the train with a box full of locusts.

0:21:44 > 0:21:48Oh, right. So these are live locusts?

0:21:48 > 0:21:51Yes, they're all munching on the grass,

0:21:51 > 0:21:54there's about six of them in there, varying sizes.

0:21:54 > 0:21:57So he's gently being placed on his back, is that right?

0:21:57 > 0:22:02Yes, in a little bed of Plasticine - it's actually a she -

0:22:02 > 0:22:05lying on her back in a bed of Plasticine

0:22:05 > 0:22:10and we've restrained the animal with little loops of Plasticine,

0:22:10 > 0:22:15and Peter is putting a very fine wire into its chest.

0:22:15 > 0:22:19The wire is rather like an acupuncture wire, very fine,

0:22:19 > 0:22:23- just a small diameter wire.- So it doesn't hurt the locust at all?

0:22:23 > 0:22:25- No, not at all.- That's amazing.

0:22:25 > 0:22:29Whilst Peter is setting up, because this is a very delicate procedure,

0:22:29 > 0:22:32let me tell you a little bit about the locust.

0:22:32 > 0:22:36The locust is really a giant kind of grasshopper, and it has this

0:22:36 > 0:22:40very simple nervous system for avoiding bumping into things.

0:22:40 > 0:22:42The reason it needs to do this is

0:22:42 > 0:22:46because when they get large in numbers they can become swarms,

0:22:46 > 0:22:48which are travelling in their vast numbers,

0:22:48 > 0:22:50and there's literally millions of them,

0:22:50 > 0:22:53and they can fly without bumping into each other.

0:22:53 > 0:22:57Swarms can be a real problem, because when they land on a crop

0:22:57 > 0:23:00they'll just ravish a crop and eat it within minutes.

0:23:00 > 0:23:02So, they are usually a pest to humans,

0:23:02 > 0:23:08but they've also been very helpful because we can measure their brain activity without hurting them.

0:23:08 > 0:23:11This is what you've been doing in your research, is that right?

0:23:11 > 0:23:15Yes, we've been looking at particular nerve cells

0:23:15 > 0:23:19within its nervous system and using this recording equipment

0:23:19 > 0:23:23to record electrical activity that the nerve cells make.

0:23:23 > 0:23:27In fact, the noise that you'll hear eventually is from

0:23:27 > 0:23:31a single nerve cell within the locust's nerve system.

0:23:31 > 0:23:33It's a very important neuron,

0:23:33 > 0:23:38one of the biggest in the body of the locust,

0:23:38 > 0:23:40and it communicates down to the wings

0:23:40 > 0:23:43and can actually shut off the flight cycle

0:23:43 > 0:23:47so that the locust will make a dive and avoid a predator

0:23:47 > 0:23:51or adjust its flight to avoid another member of the swarm.

0:23:51 > 0:23:53OK, so, Peter, are we ready?

0:23:53 > 0:23:56We're ready, I think she's ready, as well.

0:23:56 > 0:24:00I couldn't tell the difference, but I presume they're larger, the females?

0:24:00 > 0:24:02The female's larger, yes.

0:24:02 > 0:24:06OK, so which part of the visual field are we going to be recording from?

0:24:06 > 0:24:09We're recording from the right side of the locust,

0:24:09 > 0:24:13but it's the eye that is looking towards the left,

0:24:13 > 0:24:16she's on her back, so she's watching you.

0:24:16 > 0:24:18- She's watching me now? - She's watching you now.

0:24:18 > 0:24:23- Right, so we're going to listen in to her responding to me, is that correct?- Yes.

0:24:23 > 0:24:26Let's have some silence, listen very carefully,

0:24:26 > 0:24:30what you'll be hearing is the activity of the neurons.

0:24:30 > 0:24:35STATIC CRACKLING IN TIME WITH HAND MOVEMENTS

0:24:35 > 0:24:36Can you hear that?

0:24:39 > 0:24:44Do you realise you are listening to the brain of a locust?

0:24:44 > 0:24:50We know it's from that side because if I come from the other side...

0:24:53 > 0:24:54..you don't get the effect.

0:24:54 > 0:25:00Whereas, actually, I think she's paying quite a lot of attention to me now, isn't she?

0:25:02 > 0:25:06That is absolutely fascinating. Shall we try with this?

0:25:06 > 0:25:10This represents another large locust flying in the swarm towards it.

0:25:10 > 0:25:14RYTHMIC CRACKLING

0:25:14 > 0:25:16That's great.

0:25:16 > 0:25:19So, tell me, Claire, with this research what have you been able to do?

0:25:19 > 0:25:22Does it have any application for humans at all?

0:25:22 > 0:25:25It's a fascinating circuit the locust has and we've built

0:25:25 > 0:25:31an artificial circuit that we've been able to put into a sensor

0:25:31 > 0:25:34that is used for collision avoidance in cars,

0:25:34 > 0:25:38so we're hoping in the future that the circuits based on the locust

0:25:38 > 0:25:43will be able to help drivers avoid collisions in traffic.

0:25:43 > 0:25:47So, the locust insect is helping humans to avoid pileups on the M1?

0:25:47 > 0:25:49Eventually, that's what we think.

0:25:49 > 0:25:51Well, I think that's very useful.

0:25:51 > 0:25:54Can you release the locust to just show everything is fine with her?

0:25:56 > 0:25:59Can we get a close-up of the locust, just to show you?

0:25:59 > 0:26:02We can't let go because if she gets the chance she'll run away.

0:26:02 > 0:26:05You can see there she's perfectly OK.

0:26:05 > 0:26:08- And she bit me.- That's revenge, she's getting her own back, Peter.

0:26:08 > 0:26:12Let's put her back with her friends and let them go.

0:26:12 > 0:26:15Can we have a big round of applause for the locust...

0:26:15 > 0:26:16APPLAUSE

0:26:16 > 0:26:20...and Peter and Claire. Thank you so much for coming down.

0:26:27 > 0:26:29That was fascinating,

0:26:29 > 0:26:31but can we ever do the same thing for a human?

0:26:31 > 0:26:34Well, we chose a volunteer earlier, and this is Billy.

0:26:34 > 0:26:38He can't talk to us at the moment because we have him wired up.

0:26:38 > 0:26:41We're not sticking an electrode in his brain,

0:26:41 > 0:26:45rather we're recording from the outside, because it turns out

0:26:45 > 0:26:48that, if you have lots of neurons firing, they generate enough

0:26:48 > 0:26:51electrical activity that we can detect it with tiny electrodes.

0:26:51 > 0:26:55Normally when scientists do this, they have lots of electrodes,

0:26:55 > 0:27:00but for tonight's purposes we're just interested in the back of Billy's head,

0:27:00 > 0:27:02because this is where his visual area is,

0:27:02 > 0:27:04the visual cortex in the human.

0:27:04 > 0:27:08So, as before, we saw the locust was looking at a human - me -

0:27:08 > 0:27:11approaching the locust, this time we're presenting a locust

0:27:11 > 0:27:13to a human to see how they respond.

0:27:13 > 0:27:16And here we see this big pattern starting to build up.

0:27:16 > 0:27:21This is the response of Billy's visual area. Thank you, Kate.

0:27:21 > 0:27:25As you can see, as the locust was coming on it was responding,

0:27:25 > 0:27:29so the picture of the locust was generating activity in his eyes,

0:27:29 > 0:27:31then sending these impulses along the optic fibre

0:27:31 > 0:27:36to the back of the brain where the visual processing area is, then responding to that.

0:27:36 > 0:27:40That's the onset of the pattern, when the locust first appears,

0:27:40 > 0:27:43and this is the rest of the brainwave, showing how he processes it.

0:27:43 > 0:27:45So, Billy, it turns out that you do have a brain,

0:27:45 > 0:27:50so thank you very much and a big round of applause to Kate and Billy.

0:27:50 > 0:27:53APPLAUSE AND CHEERING

0:27:53 > 0:27:58Animals might be all moving and interacting in the same environment,

0:27:58 > 0:28:02like the locusts and humans, but their brains are very different,

0:28:02 > 0:28:05and what they're experiencing must be different.

0:28:05 > 0:28:09And even our own experience is often not what it seems.

0:28:09 > 0:28:11So, consider vision again.

0:28:11 > 0:28:15Most of us think that vision is rich and full of detail -

0:28:15 > 0:28:18in fact, a lot of us think it's almost like a camera, but is it really?

0:28:18 > 0:28:22Let's test that idea out. Joe, would you come in here?

0:28:22 > 0:28:26Joe's got a camera on his shoulder and he's taking the image

0:28:26 > 0:28:29and you can see that the image is projected above me

0:28:29 > 0:28:31and it's nice and rich and full of detail.

0:28:31 > 0:28:34This is what we think vision is really like,

0:28:34 > 0:28:37but, actually, human vision isn't like that at all,

0:28:37 > 0:28:40because we know from the studies using these techniques

0:28:40 > 0:28:43you're only ever processing the centre part of your vision,

0:28:43 > 0:28:46and in fact it's about the size of your thumb held at arm's length.

0:28:46 > 0:28:50So, can we make the camera appear like human vision?

0:28:50 > 0:28:52Now you can see it's all blurred at the edge

0:28:52 > 0:28:56and it's only the central part of the field which is clear and detailed.

0:28:56 > 0:29:01That's a bit strange, because that's not the way you experience vision, do you?

0:29:01 > 0:29:03You see it as full and complex.

0:29:03 > 0:29:07So, why is that? Well, let me show you.

0:29:07 > 0:29:09If I move a bit closer...

0:29:09 > 0:29:12the way that it seems more detailed is, of course,

0:29:12 > 0:29:15I simply move my eyes around.

0:29:15 > 0:29:20And I'm moving them quite rapidly, about four or five times per second.

0:29:20 > 0:29:21These are called saccades.

0:29:21 > 0:29:24This is how the brain builds up a picture of complexity,

0:29:24 > 0:29:26because you're sampling the world

0:29:26 > 0:29:30then storing that information, and the brain is remembering it,

0:29:30 > 0:29:33and this is what makes the world seem much more complex.

0:29:33 > 0:29:35There's a problem, though,

0:29:35 > 0:29:38because if a camera was to move like human vision,

0:29:38 > 0:29:41there'd be a real distortion, so let's take that away.

0:29:41 > 0:29:44Joe, can you move your camera like an eye movement?

0:29:44 > 0:29:46Let's see what that looks like.

0:29:55 > 0:29:59Now, what's wrong with that? Any suggestions? Yes.

0:29:59 > 0:30:01- AUDIENCE MEMBER:- It's kind of moving quite fast.

0:30:01 > 0:30:04It's very jerky, isn't it? It's very blurred.

0:30:04 > 0:30:07If that was your normal vision it'd make you very seasick,

0:30:07 > 0:30:10so your brain does a very clever trick.

0:30:10 > 0:30:14Every time you move your eyes it cuts out the visual information,

0:30:14 > 0:30:17so you don't see all those jerky, smeared, blurred images.

0:30:17 > 0:30:21So, Joe, can you simulate that, can you turn off the visual signal

0:30:21 > 0:30:23every time you're moving the camera?

0:30:29 > 0:30:35OK, your brain is literally cutting off all the visual information, in fact you can't see anything.

0:30:35 > 0:30:39We know that's true, I'm going to prove it and I'll need a volunteer.

0:30:39 > 0:30:42Let's see who we can choose. Do we have anyone? Young lady.

0:30:42 > 0:30:44- What's your name?- Amy.

0:30:44 > 0:30:47Amy, OK, would you hold the mirror like this, OK?

0:30:47 > 0:30:49Josh, can you pick up Amy? Good.

0:30:49 > 0:30:52Now, Amy, have a look at your left eye.

0:30:52 > 0:30:54Now look at your right eye.

0:30:54 > 0:30:56Swap backwards and forwards.

0:30:56 > 0:31:00- Can you see your eyes moving?- No.

0:31:01 > 0:31:04Can you see your eyes moving at all?

0:31:04 > 0:31:06- Can anyone else see her eyes moving? - AUDIENCE:- Yes.

0:31:06 > 0:31:10Amy, your eyes are moving, would you like me to prove it to you?

0:31:10 > 0:31:12Have a look up there.

0:31:12 > 0:31:14Ready?

0:31:17 > 0:31:19LAUGHTER

0:31:19 > 0:31:22It would seem like you're surprised, but don't worry,

0:31:22 > 0:31:25you're perfectly normal, you can't see your own eyes moving at all.

0:31:25 > 0:31:28Applause for Amy, please.

0:31:28 > 0:31:31APPLAUSE

0:31:33 > 0:31:36Now, you can try that all at home, actually.

0:31:36 > 0:31:39If you're brushing your teeth, just look in the mirror

0:31:39 > 0:31:42then focus on your left eye and shift to your right eye,

0:31:42 > 0:31:45see if you can see your eyes moving, and you won't be able to,

0:31:45 > 0:31:48because, no matter how you try, your brain is making you blind.

0:31:48 > 0:31:54Effectively, if you add up all the gaps - you're moving your eyes all the time when you're awake -

0:31:54 > 0:31:59you're blind for about two hours of the day, and you never even know that. Isn't that remarkable?

0:31:59 > 0:32:03Clearly the mind has amazing tricks that keep the world looking

0:32:03 > 0:32:06rich and full of detail and information.

0:32:06 > 0:32:09So, what happens to all that information once you've detected it?

0:32:09 > 0:32:12Ice creams! Get your ice creams here! Ice creams!

0:32:12 > 0:32:16Ah, here's some rich information - an ice cream. Thank you very much.

0:32:16 > 0:32:22So, consider an ice cream. It's full of lots of information.

0:32:22 > 0:32:27It looks delicious, it smells delicious, I'm going to do this...

0:32:28 > 0:32:34You can hear the crunch, it's cold, and it tastes very yummy,

0:32:34 > 0:32:38but somehow my brain combines all these different sensations

0:32:38 > 0:32:41into one experience a delicious ice cream.

0:32:41 > 0:32:42How does it do that?

0:32:42 > 0:32:48I'm going to show you by building a very simple brain in this auditorium.

0:32:48 > 0:32:52OK, we had some helmets given out earlier, so pop your helmets on.

0:32:52 > 0:32:56Those with the helmets, would you mind standing up?

0:32:56 > 0:32:58these are our volunteers.

0:32:58 > 0:33:01You're going to represent different groups of neurons.

0:33:01 > 0:33:05Let's say this part of the brain is coding for shapes.

0:33:05 > 0:33:08Sir, at the back, you code for anything which is round.

0:33:08 > 0:33:11And you in the front code for anything which is long, like a pencil.

0:33:11 > 0:33:15This part of the audience will represent the part of the brain

0:33:15 > 0:33:18which codes for colour, so you're going to be green,

0:33:18 > 0:33:21and you're going to respond to anything that's yellow.

0:33:21 > 0:33:24And over here we have a part of the brain which codes for taste,

0:33:24 > 0:33:28so you're getting information from the mouth, and you at the back,

0:33:28 > 0:33:31you're going to be sweet and you're going to be salty.

0:33:31 > 0:33:35Now hold-up these connections, because these are going to stand

0:33:35 > 0:33:40for all the connections between the different regions of the brain.

0:33:40 > 0:33:44Now press your buttons and let's see all the activity in the brain sending signals.

0:33:44 > 0:33:47You can flash them, let's see a lot of random connections.

0:33:47 > 0:33:51So here's our simple brain. How does a simple brain learn about objects?

0:33:51 > 0:33:54Well, we're going to teach it to learn about fruit.

0:33:54 > 0:33:58OK, so pop your lights off for a moment.

0:33:58 > 0:34:01Now, imagine that you've never eaten a banana before.

0:34:01 > 0:34:05So, let's have you responding, if your feature's present, hold down your button.

0:34:05 > 0:34:07Let's see what that looks like.

0:34:07 > 0:34:09So it's long, it's yellow,

0:34:09 > 0:34:12and you pop it in your mouth and what does it taste like? Sweet.

0:34:12 > 0:34:15Sweet. So that's the pattern for a banana.

0:34:15 > 0:34:19All right, now everyone switch your lights on again,

0:34:19 > 0:34:23communicating again, there it's talking to itself, and put them off.

0:34:23 > 0:34:26Let's come across another fruit.

0:34:26 > 0:34:29This time, it's round, hold it down...

0:34:29 > 0:34:31it's green...

0:34:31 > 0:34:34and you pop it into your mouth and it's sweet.

0:34:36 > 0:34:39And every time you eat a banana, or a grape,

0:34:39 > 0:34:41that pattern becomes stronger.

0:34:41 > 0:34:45This is because the neurons that fire together are wiring together.

0:34:45 > 0:34:50Now, you might notice how the banana and the grape

0:34:50 > 0:34:53are activating the same part, which is the sweet centres.

0:34:53 > 0:34:56That shows you the brain can reuse the same regions

0:34:56 > 0:34:57to code for different objects.

0:34:57 > 0:35:01So, what happens when you go to a new part of the world

0:35:01 > 0:35:04and encounter new food you've never had before?

0:35:04 > 0:35:06Turn your lights off for a moment.

0:35:06 > 0:35:08Let's say you go to the Mediterranean

0:35:08 > 0:35:11and you see this small, round, green thing.

0:35:11 > 0:35:15So it's round, it's green, and your brain thinks,

0:35:15 > 0:35:18"Well, it looks like a grape, so it's going to be sweet."

0:35:18 > 0:35:20So, sweet, pop your light on.

0:35:20 > 0:35:23But then when you pop it into your mouth...

0:35:23 > 0:35:26Yuck! It's salty!

0:35:26 > 0:35:28So, this is why you can be very surprised

0:35:28 > 0:35:31when you encounter something new which seems so familiar.

0:35:31 > 0:35:35That's why new foods can surprise you.

0:35:35 > 0:35:38Let's consider our simple brain again.

0:35:38 > 0:35:41If I show you this pattern...

0:35:41 > 0:35:45Tell us, audience, what do you think that stands for, that pattern?

0:35:45 > 0:35:47AUDIENCE: Banana.

0:35:47 > 0:35:51It stands for banana, but it's not really a banana, is it?

0:35:51 > 0:35:54It's just how the brain recreates the sensation of eating bananas.

0:35:54 > 0:35:57It's what we call a representation,

0:35:57 > 0:36:01because the brain is re-presenting the original experience.

0:36:01 > 0:36:04Representations are really the language of the brain.

0:36:04 > 0:36:09Now, I have given you a very simple demonstration with only a few groups of neurons,

0:36:09 > 0:36:11just to give you an idea of different patterns,

0:36:11 > 0:36:13but the brain is much more complex.

0:36:13 > 0:36:17If this were a real brain, there'd be 100 billion neurons,

0:36:17 > 0:36:20and you wouldn't just be holding a couple of connections,

0:36:20 > 0:36:24because the neurons have up to 10,000 connections between them,

0:36:24 > 0:36:26and if you add up all the connections end to end,

0:36:26 > 0:36:30that stretches to 180,000 kilometres,

0:36:30 > 0:36:33and that's long enough to stretch around the world four times.

0:36:33 > 0:36:35That's rather mind blowing, isn't it?

0:36:35 > 0:36:38Because it means your brain has the capacity to encode an almost

0:36:38 > 0:36:42infinite number of patterns, which is why we say the human brain

0:36:42 > 0:36:45is the most complex structure to be found in nature.

0:36:45 > 0:36:49So, let's have a big round of applause for our small brain.

0:36:49 > 0:36:53APPLAUSE

0:36:54 > 0:36:59So, our world is full of rich experiences that are combined into these meaningful patterns,

0:36:59 > 0:37:03and these representations reflect all the structure and order

0:37:03 > 0:37:08that we encounter on a regular basis in our existence, in our lives.

0:37:08 > 0:37:11So, for example, if I have this garbage lid,

0:37:11 > 0:37:14you're processing this in different parts of your brain.

0:37:14 > 0:37:17You have a areas which are processing the vision,

0:37:17 > 0:37:19and if I drop it...

0:37:19 > 0:37:22you have areas of your brain processing the sound.

0:37:22 > 0:37:25So your visual area's active, and so are your sound areas.

0:37:25 > 0:37:30In fact, you've got a set of neurons which combine that experience of sight and sound.

0:37:30 > 0:37:33This representation of sight and sound is usually quite reliable,

0:37:33 > 0:37:36because sights usually go with sounds, but sometimes

0:37:36 > 0:37:40it can lead you to some false and surprising conclusions.

0:37:42 > 0:37:45BELCHING SOUNDS

0:37:45 > 0:37:48Let's try that one more time.

0:37:48 > 0:37:50BELCHING SOUNDS

0:37:50 > 0:37:54So, what you're doing there is you think that the skull is burping,

0:37:54 > 0:37:57but of course he isn't really, what's happening

0:37:57 > 0:38:01is you're seeing the skull move and you're hearing the sound

0:38:01 > 0:38:04and your brain's readily putting those things together.

0:38:04 > 0:38:06This is called the ventriloquist effect.

0:38:06 > 0:38:10And so, when people see ventriloquists they think they're throwing their voice,

0:38:10 > 0:38:13but they're not really throwing their voice,

0:38:13 > 0:38:16they're minimising the movement of their own mouths,

0:38:16 > 0:38:19making a sound, and exaggerating the mouth of the puppet.

0:38:19 > 0:38:22I can do that for a little bit, let's see if I can try

0:38:22 > 0:38:24and convince you this skull is talking,

0:38:24 > 0:38:29So if I go, she sells seashells on the seashore.

0:38:29 > 0:38:30OK.

0:38:30 > 0:38:35Not only does a ventriloquist shape where you think a sound's coming from,

0:38:35 > 0:38:38because sights and sounds usually come from the same place,

0:38:38 > 0:38:42the ventriloquist effect can also influence what you're hearing.

0:38:42 > 0:38:45So, in this next example, I want you to watch very carefully

0:38:45 > 0:38:49this little bit of video and see if you can hear what I'm saying.

0:38:51 > 0:38:55REPEATS EITHER "BA-BA! OR "DA-DA"

0:38:55 > 0:38:57What did you hear?

0:38:57 > 0:39:00AUDIENCE GIVES A MIXED RESPONSE

0:39:00 > 0:39:03Who heard "da-da", put your hand up?

0:39:03 > 0:39:06OK, everybody sitting in the middle. Let's try it again.

0:39:06 > 0:39:10REPEATS EITHER 'BA-BA' OR 'DA-DA'

0:39:10 > 0:39:12What do you hear? "Da-da?"

0:39:12 > 0:39:14Middle section only, what did you hear?

0:39:14 > 0:39:16"Da?" "Ba?"

0:39:16 > 0:39:18MIXED RESPONSE

0:39:18 > 0:39:20All right, let's make this easier.

0:39:20 > 0:39:24I want you to listen again, but close your eyes, OK?

0:39:26 > 0:39:30REPEATS EITHER "BA-BA" OR "DA-DA"

0:39:30 > 0:39:32What did you hear this time?

0:39:32 > 0:39:33AUDIENCE: "Ba".

0:39:33 > 0:39:34Definitely it was "Ba".

0:39:34 > 0:39:39If you heard "da" you were being fooled by an illusion called the McGurk effect,

0:39:39 > 0:39:42because I'm not actually mouthing "ba-ba" or "da-da",

0:39:42 > 0:39:45what I'm mouthing is "ga-ga".

0:39:45 > 0:39:48So the brain gets the signal of "ga", and it's hearing "ba",

0:39:48 > 0:39:51but these are patterns it's never encountered before,

0:39:51 > 0:39:54and it comes up with a solution which is "da".

0:39:54 > 0:39:58So your brain's always trying to interpret experiences

0:39:58 > 0:40:00to come up with the best solution.

0:40:00 > 0:40:03Now, this way when you're watching people speak,

0:40:03 > 0:40:06you watch their mouths moving and the shape of their mouths

0:40:06 > 0:40:08can influence what you think you're hearing.

0:40:08 > 0:40:10Here's a very simple party trick -

0:40:10 > 0:40:13I want you to turn to the person next to you

0:40:13 > 0:40:17and mouth the words "elephant juice". Don't say it, just mouth it.

0:40:17 > 0:40:19Turn to the person next to you.

0:40:23 > 0:40:27What does it sound like? What do you think the person might be saying?

0:40:27 > 0:40:30Why are you laughing?

0:40:30 > 0:40:32OK, OK.

0:40:32 > 0:40:35Does it look like they could be saying "I love you"?

0:40:35 > 0:40:37Do I hear "I love you"?

0:40:37 > 0:40:40I love you all, wouldn't the world be a greater place

0:40:40 > 0:40:45if everyone said "elephant juice" a little more often to each other?

0:40:45 > 0:40:50OK, so your brain is not just forming representations

0:40:50 > 0:40:55of the outside world, it's also storing these representations of your own bodies,

0:40:55 > 0:40:59and so for this next demonstration I'm going to require someone

0:40:59 > 0:41:01who doesn't mind losing their hand.

0:41:02 > 0:41:06Lady on the end here, why don't you come down?

0:41:06 > 0:41:10APPLAUSE

0:41:10 > 0:41:14It's all right, just there. What's your name, first of all?

0:41:14 > 0:41:18- Josie.- Josie, so you're quite prepared to lose your hand

0:41:18 > 0:41:22for medical science, is that correct? You don't mind losing a hand?

0:41:22 > 0:41:24Don't worry, I'm not going to remove your hand,

0:41:24 > 0:41:28I'm going to create the illusion that you're losing your hand.

0:41:28 > 0:41:31So, for this I need you to put on this very strange jacket.

0:41:31 > 0:41:33It's actually got three arms.

0:41:33 > 0:41:37OK, so put your arm through that one, then through the other one.

0:41:37 > 0:41:39That's the regular part of the jacket.

0:41:39 > 0:41:42Is that going to be a bit small for you? Perfect.

0:41:42 > 0:41:46- Now, we're going to torture you. OK, so now, Josie, did you say?- Yes.

0:41:46 > 0:41:49OK, Josie, take a seat. I want you to put your other hand

0:41:49 > 0:41:52up here, your left hand, both hands are there.

0:41:52 > 0:41:55OK, now, that looks a little bit strange,

0:41:55 > 0:41:59but I want you just to focus on this, this is a rubber hand.

0:41:59 > 0:42:02It's about the same size as Josie's hand, and I want you

0:42:02 > 0:42:05not to look at the audience, just concentrate on the hand, OK?

0:42:05 > 0:42:09This illusion takes about a minute or two to form.

0:42:09 > 0:42:13What should happen is that Josie is looking at this hand of hers,

0:42:13 > 0:42:16and it's in the same place her normal hand is.

0:42:16 > 0:42:22So, her brain is a little bit confused, because that hand should belong to her.

0:42:22 > 0:42:25At the same time, to make the illusion even more strong,

0:42:25 > 0:42:28Kate is simultaneously stroking the hands,

0:42:28 > 0:42:32so the brain is now receiving all this touch information.

0:42:32 > 0:42:36Again, it's combining information, trying to make sense of it.

0:42:36 > 0:42:39- How does that feel? - It feels really weird.

0:42:39 > 0:42:42- Does it feel very weird?- Yeah. - OK.

0:42:42 > 0:42:44So, just keep doing that for a moment.

0:42:49 > 0:42:53Now just keep focusing on the hand, OK?

0:42:53 > 0:42:54Ready?

0:42:55 > 0:42:58Did you get a strange... Did that feel a bit odd?

0:42:58 > 0:43:00OK, don't worry, I wasn't going to hurt your hand.

0:43:00 > 0:43:02Round of applause.

0:43:02 > 0:43:06APPLAUSE

0:43:10 > 0:43:12Thank you very much. Well done.

0:43:12 > 0:43:15Now, the reason that happened is because the brain wasn't

0:43:15 > 0:43:18exactly sure whether it was the rubber hand any more,

0:43:18 > 0:43:22and that's why most people you do that experiment with get this surprise.

0:43:22 > 0:43:26I can't bring you all down to try the rubber hand illusion,

0:43:26 > 0:43:28but I can show you a similar experience,

0:43:28 > 0:43:31so I'll need another volunteer, someone from this side.

0:43:31 > 0:43:33Young lady in the blue, why don't you come down?

0:43:35 > 0:43:38APPLAUSE

0:43:38 > 0:43:40- What's your name?- Charlotte.

0:43:40 > 0:43:44OK, Charlotte, here's a very simple way to induce the rubber hand illusion.

0:43:44 > 0:43:46I'm just going to turn you this way for a bit.

0:43:46 > 0:43:49There you go, Charlotte. Now put up your right hand.

0:43:49 > 0:43:51OK.

0:43:51 > 0:43:55Now, with your other hand just grip like this.

0:43:55 > 0:44:00Now, looking at your index finger, just move your fingers up and down whilst you're doing it.

0:44:00 > 0:44:03- Does that feel a little strange? You can all try this.- Yes.

0:44:03 > 0:44:06Just turn to the person next to you.

0:44:06 > 0:44:10With your thumb and forefinger see if you can do this.

0:44:12 > 0:44:15It's strange, isn't it? It is very, very weird.

0:44:15 > 0:44:18You can try it at home as well, if you like.

0:44:24 > 0:44:27All right, guys, let's settle down.

0:44:27 > 0:44:30Let's give a round of applause to Charlotte.

0:44:30 > 0:44:34- APPLAUSE - Thank you, Charlotte.

0:44:38 > 0:44:41So, your brain is always trying to make sense of the world.

0:44:41 > 0:44:46Sometimes, when it gets strange signals, it comes up with strange experiences and illusions.

0:44:46 > 0:44:53We see things all the time. It might be faces in a cloud or animals in ink stains.

0:44:53 > 0:44:55Just simple coffee beans.

0:44:55 > 0:45:01If I scatter them onto here, you can see all sorts of patterns in that.

0:45:01 > 0:45:05Can anyone see a pattern forming there at all?

0:45:05 > 0:45:07Shout if you see anything.

0:45:07 > 0:45:09There's a mouse?

0:45:09 > 0:45:12SHOUTING FROM AUDIENCE

0:45:12 > 0:45:13What else?

0:45:15 > 0:45:16Oh...

0:45:16 > 0:45:19AUDIENCE SHOUTS

0:45:19 > 0:45:20OK.

0:45:22 > 0:45:27So, you're all seeing lots of patterns. That's very good.

0:45:27 > 0:45:31OK. Clearly you're all seeing lots of things. That's very reassuring.

0:45:31 > 0:45:34Your brain always tries to impose structure and order.

0:45:34 > 0:45:38This is most obvious with certain types of illusions

0:45:38 > 0:45:42where you have patterns which can be seen in more than one way.

0:45:42 > 0:45:46Probably one of the most famous examples is called The Necker Cube.

0:45:46 > 0:45:48Here is a Necker Cube.

0:45:48 > 0:45:53It is an outline of a cube. If you look at it long enough, you think it's pointing in one direction

0:45:53 > 0:45:57but then, if you stare at it long enough, your brain switches

0:45:57 > 0:46:01and it appears to be in the opposite direction.

0:46:01 > 0:46:03Is anyone having that experience? Hands up.

0:46:03 > 0:46:05That's great.

0:46:05 > 0:46:07We can make it stronger if we put a bit of movement into it.

0:46:07 > 0:46:11So just watch as it turns.

0:46:11 > 0:46:18It seems to be going in one direction. Then, is anyone getting it turning in the other direction?

0:46:18 > 0:46:23Yeah? Just watch. You know what makes it really good?

0:46:23 > 0:46:26If you blink, while you're watching, you'll see it switch.

0:46:26 > 0:46:30We're not using any computer trickery here.

0:46:30 > 0:46:34It's simply your brain switching from one version versus the other.

0:46:36 > 0:46:38Isn't that remarkable?

0:46:38 > 0:46:40AUDIENCE MURMURS

0:46:40 > 0:46:42Here is another interesting point.

0:46:42 > 0:46:47Your brain doesn't allow you to see all the patterns at once.

0:46:47 > 0:46:50It forces you into one perception versus the other.

0:46:50 > 0:46:56This might explain why some of you sometimes see things like ghosts, for example.

0:46:56 > 0:47:00I am going to conjure up a ghost in front of your very eyes, OK?

0:47:00 > 0:47:06Don't worry, it won't be a headless horseman. It'll be more simple and friendly than that.

0:47:06 > 0:47:07All you need for this,

0:47:07 > 0:47:11and you can try this at home, are just four circles of paper.

0:47:11 > 0:47:14It's such a simple illusion but it's very compelling.

0:47:18 > 0:47:22All I have to do his cut a quarter of the circle.

0:47:29 > 0:47:32If you then align up the circles...

0:47:34 > 0:47:39..you will see something that isn't really there.

0:47:40 > 0:47:43What does anyone see? Hands up. Shout out.

0:47:43 > 0:47:46- AUDIENCE SHOUTS - That's right. A square.

0:47:46 > 0:47:49But, of course, there isn't a square there, is there?

0:47:49 > 0:47:53When I take this away it disappears and back it comes again.

0:47:53 > 0:47:57This is a very simple illusion, but it's also a very powerful one

0:47:57 > 0:48:02because I think it explains one of the most important points about the brain.

0:48:02 > 0:48:08If I went into the back of your brain with a wire, I could measure activity

0:48:08 > 0:48:12of neurons which are firing as if they're really was a square there.

0:48:12 > 0:48:18So this is remarkable - the brain is creating its own experience, isn't it?

0:48:18 > 0:48:23In fact, we can even show that you think this is a solid object.

0:48:23 > 0:48:25They've done this recently in an experiment

0:48:25 > 0:48:30where they've put people in a brain scanner and they've showed them this square.

0:48:30 > 0:48:32It's called the Kanizsa Illusion.

0:48:33 > 0:48:37Here we have the square. Then they made the square move.

0:48:39 > 0:48:43You see, it is travelling across.

0:48:43 > 0:48:47Watch as it moves across the screen.

0:48:50 > 0:48:53There it goes again. Undulating like a real object.

0:48:55 > 0:48:57Isn't that very bizarre?

0:48:59 > 0:49:03AUDIENCE MURMURS

0:49:03 > 0:49:08It's moving across the screen. See if we can move it a bit faster.

0:49:08 > 0:49:10There it goes, moving across the screen.

0:49:15 > 0:49:19What's remarkable is that the movement areas of the brain are being activated

0:49:19 > 0:49:24which are going in the same direction as the illusory ghostly square.

0:49:24 > 0:49:28So, your brain doesn't allow you to have contact with reality.

0:49:28 > 0:49:31It is generating reality the whole time.

0:49:31 > 0:49:33It is quite remarkable.

0:49:33 > 0:49:37Let us come back to Charlie and Iona, at the beginning.

0:49:37 > 0:49:40Let's get them back in for the rest of the show.

0:49:40 > 0:49:42Where are you? Come one down.

0:49:42 > 0:49:44A big round of applause, please.

0:49:44 > 0:49:47APPLAUSE

0:49:50 > 0:49:53- So, guys, how are you enjoying the show so far?- It is great.

0:49:53 > 0:49:55Reality hasn't changed for you. You're still very tall, Charlie,

0:49:55 > 0:50:01- and you're still A bit shorter, aren't you?- Yes.- Would you like to be taller than Charlie?- Yes.

0:50:01 > 0:50:05Well, with the RI Christmas show we can actually make that happen

0:50:05 > 0:50:08so please follow Kate out of the room for a moment.

0:50:08 > 0:50:11We will be seeing them very shortly.

0:50:11 > 0:50:17Throughout the lecture tonight, we've been watching how reality is created by the brain.

0:50:17 > 0:50:21And it uses past experiences to make sense of the world.

0:50:21 > 0:50:25But things are not always as they seem.

0:50:25 > 0:50:28Sometimes we can fool the...

0:50:28 > 0:50:30Oh! Hello, guys.

0:50:30 > 0:50:32There we go! Can you give a wave, Charlie.

0:50:32 > 0:50:36- Charlie, that room seems a bit small, doesn't it?- Yeah.

0:50:36 > 0:50:40Maybe you should try going to the opposite corner.

0:50:40 > 0:50:42- Iona, why don't you switch places? - OK.- OK.

0:50:44 > 0:50:49Oh, my gosh! How did that happen? Switch places again.

0:50:54 > 0:50:57Can you hold hands? Can you reach each other?

0:50:57 > 0:50:59There we go.

0:50:59 > 0:51:02Look, it's a giant and a smaller person.

0:51:03 > 0:51:07OK. You can have the bit of fun in there because I'll explain what's going on.

0:51:07 > 0:51:10To do that, I need a model.

0:51:10 > 0:51:13What you can't see is that's obviously not a normal room.

0:51:13 > 0:51:17In fact, the room has this sort of shape.

0:51:17 > 0:51:20It is just the way we have set the camera angle.

0:51:20 > 0:51:27What week doing is fooling and tricking your brain into thinking that is, in fact, a square room.

0:51:27 > 0:51:30I can illustrate this with the next example over here.

0:51:33 > 0:51:36Do these lines look equally long to you?

0:51:36 > 0:51:38AUDIENCE: Yes.

0:51:38 > 0:51:41The green lines? Who says they look the same?

0:51:41 > 0:51:45That is a very strange brain you have. The rest of you, I hope...

0:51:45 > 0:51:47Who things they look longer?

0:51:48 > 0:51:53That's great! Because, of course, it's an illusion.

0:51:53 > 0:51:55This is the Ponzo Illusion.

0:51:55 > 0:51:58In fact, the lines are exactly the same length.

0:52:00 > 0:52:02I'm going to make that big again.

0:52:02 > 0:52:08What is going on here is your brain has been fooled by what are called perspective cues.

0:52:08 > 0:52:10It's almost like it's on a railway track

0:52:10 > 0:52:15and because railway tracks recede off into the distance, they converge.

0:52:15 > 0:52:18Because this seems to be further away

0:52:18 > 0:52:22and it is stretching over the edge, we assume it must be much larger

0:52:22 > 0:52:25than this block which is sitting inside the tracks.

0:52:25 > 0:52:28Once again, even though your brain tells you that they look different

0:52:28 > 0:52:32in fact they are exactly the same length.

0:52:34 > 0:52:37So, at this is what is going on in the Ames Room.

0:52:37 > 0:52:41It uses these perspective cues of slanted lines to fool your brain

0:52:41 > 0:52:45into thinking that the room is actually the same distance.

0:52:45 > 0:52:47In fact, it is actually longer.

0:52:47 > 0:52:50I suppose the best way to show you how it will work is if I go out there

0:52:50 > 0:52:55and you can see what I am like and what the room is really like. OK?

0:53:00 > 0:53:03- Here we are at the Ames Room. How are you doing?- Fine.

0:53:03 > 0:53:04If you have a look a round,

0:53:04 > 0:53:07you can see that the room the room isn't straight or normal.

0:53:07 > 0:53:11It has slanting lines and the way the team have built it...

0:53:11 > 0:53:15when you shoot it from one angle it looks as if the perspective is correct,

0:53:15 > 0:53:18but in fact it is entirely wrong. Why don't you swap over again?

0:53:18 > 0:53:21In fact, I think I'll join you.

0:53:21 > 0:53:23So you can watch me going into the room

0:53:23 > 0:53:25and look how I transform in size. Here I come!

0:53:25 > 0:53:27So, I'm big.

0:53:30 > 0:53:34And now, Iona, you're bigger than both of us.

0:53:34 > 0:53:37So, that's part of the magic of the Royal Institution.

0:53:37 > 0:53:42Why don't you come back in and give a big round of applause for everyone?

0:53:42 > 0:53:44APPLAUSE

0:53:53 > 0:53:55So, all these illusions demonstrate

0:53:55 > 0:53:58our brains constantly try to make sense of the world

0:53:58 > 0:54:01and understand based on these stored representations.

0:54:01 > 0:54:04The remarkable thing about illusions is, even when you know how they work,

0:54:04 > 0:54:07and I've just demonstrated with the models and shown

0:54:07 > 0:54:11that these are illusions, you can't help but see them one way or the other.

0:54:11 > 0:54:14It's because your brain is creating your mind's experience.

0:54:14 > 0:54:15You can't avoid that.

0:54:15 > 0:54:19So, you remember I promised you we were going to look at Thalia's brain

0:54:19 > 0:54:24and read her mind? Let's go back to Cheltenham and see if we've made that link.

0:54:24 > 0:54:27Hello, Cheltenham, can you hear me at all?

0:54:27 > 0:54:30- It's Cheltenham here. Hi, London. - Is that Iain, is it?

0:54:30 > 0:54:32Yes, hi, Bruce, hi!

0:54:32 > 0:54:36Hi, hi! How's it been going? Have you managed to scan Thalia's brain?

0:54:36 > 0:54:38Yeah, we've got some great pictures. It looks good.

0:54:38 > 0:54:42Good. Can you send through the first image so we can get an idea of what you've got.

0:54:42 > 0:54:45- That's structural image, is that correct?- Yeah.

0:54:45 > 0:54:51So, I'm going to tell the audience, to give them an idea what they're looking at.

0:54:51 > 0:54:55Imagine I'm Thalia and I'm lying inside the scanner. Here I am.

0:54:55 > 0:55:00And so the scan is going from the bottom of my brain up to the top of my head.

0:55:00 > 0:55:05So this side of the screen is the right side of my brain, OK?

0:55:05 > 0:55:10This side of the image on this side of the screen is the left side of my brain.

0:55:10 > 0:55:16So, Iain, am I correct? Did you try showing Thalia a visual image earlier, is that right?

0:55:16 > 0:55:19Yes, we did. We showed her a visual object, yeah.

0:55:19 > 0:55:23Can you show us what the brain activation was like

0:55:23 > 0:55:26when she was looking at a visual object? OK.

0:55:26 > 0:55:30So, tell us, what part of the brain is that that's being activated?

0:55:30 > 0:55:35- The back.- So, if it's the back part of the brain, what's going on?

0:55:35 > 0:55:39- Which area's that?- Vision.- That's right. We showed Thalia a picture.

0:55:39 > 0:55:43When she looked at the picture, the back of her brain was more active.

0:55:43 > 0:55:45This was showing the functions of her brain working.

0:55:45 > 0:55:49Now, Thalia, we asked her to...

0:55:49 > 0:55:52we had a walnut and we asked her to put it in one of her hands.

0:55:52 > 0:55:54Have you been processing that image, Iain?

0:55:54 > 0:55:57Yes, we have. We've been processing the image.

0:55:57 > 0:56:02You say she had a walnut in one hand. We took some pictures while she was squeezing the walnut with her hand.

0:56:02 > 0:56:05Great. Have you got those images ready for us? OK.

0:56:05 > 0:56:10So, if that's the image, which side of the brain is more active? Which side of the image is it?

0:56:10 > 0:56:13- Right.- So, it's on the right side.

0:56:13 > 0:56:16This is the right side of my brain. What you've learned tonight

0:56:16 > 0:56:20about how things cross over, which hand is Thalia holding the walnut in?

0:56:20 > 0:56:26- Left!- Thalia, could you confirm to me which hand you were squeezing the walnut with?

0:56:26 > 0:56:29My left hand.

0:56:29 > 0:56:32Thank you.

0:56:33 > 0:56:36You have just mind read Thalia because you predicted which hand

0:56:36 > 0:56:41she was holding it in. Do you realise that's over 100 miles away?

0:56:41 > 0:56:43A big round of applause, everyone.

0:56:43 > 0:56:45APPLAUSE

0:56:53 > 0:56:57Before we go, can we say thank you and good night to Thalia and Iain? Good night, Cheltenham.

0:56:57 > 0:56:59Good night.

0:56:59 > 0:57:01So, that's what's inside your head.

0:57:01 > 0:57:06Your brain is interpreting the world around you into meaningful patterns

0:57:06 > 0:57:10and storing those patterns of representations. With these technologies

0:57:10 > 0:57:12we can read the activity of the brain.

0:57:12 > 0:57:16Does that mean, to know what's on someone's mind, we have to look at brain activity?

0:57:16 > 0:57:19The technologies are useful if you know what you're looking for

0:57:19 > 0:57:24and the tasks are very simple, like squeezing a walnut in one hand.

0:57:24 > 0:57:27But, the thing about humans is, we're very complicated.

0:57:27 > 0:57:29The tasks we can do are very difficult.

0:57:29 > 0:57:31That's what makes us human in many ways.

0:57:31 > 0:57:34So, that raises the question,

0:57:34 > 0:57:38who was coordinating all these difficult tasks and activities?

0:57:38 > 0:57:43Who is in charge anyway? We'll be addressing that in the next lecture.

0:57:43 > 0:57:46Good night and look after your brains. Good night.

0:57:46 > 0:57:50CHEERING AND APPLAUSE