Browse content similar to The Secret Life of the Dog. Check below for episodes and series from the same categories and more!
Line | From | To | |
---|---|---|---|
We have an extraordinary relationship with dogs. | 0:00:04 | 0:00:07 | |
We love them like no other animal on the planet. | 0:00:07 | 0:00:11 | |
What makes our relationship so special, is perhaps the dog's | 0:00:11 | 0:00:14 | |
ability to be able to read our emotions so effectively. | 0:00:14 | 0:00:18 | |
They've been around longer than any other pet. | 0:00:18 | 0:00:22 | |
There are now eight million dogs living in the UK alone. | 0:00:22 | 0:00:25 | |
The dogs are wonderful. | 0:00:27 | 0:00:29 | |
We've got over 400 breeds across the world and every one of them has something special about them. | 0:00:29 | 0:00:34 | |
But only now are we beginning to realise just how important that relationship could be. | 0:00:37 | 0:00:43 | |
New research is revealing ever more | 0:00:46 | 0:00:49 | |
intricate connections between human and dog. | 0:00:49 | 0:00:53 | |
Without that initial starting phase of dog domestication, civilisation just would not have been possible. | 0:00:53 | 0:00:58 | |
So why do we love an animal that was once a fearsome predator? | 0:01:01 | 0:01:05 | |
TRANSLATION: This fire-breathing dragon has turned into a human friend. | 0:01:07 | 0:01:12 | |
Could they in some ways be more intelligent than even our closest relative? | 0:01:12 | 0:01:16 | |
Suddenly, there were dogs doing something that not even chimps could do. | 0:01:16 | 0:01:19 | |
And in the future, what impact might dogs have on all our lives? | 0:01:19 | 0:01:25 | |
They're going to help us tackle some of the most dangerous diseases of | 0:01:25 | 0:01:29 | |
our time, diseases that are killing millions of people every year. | 0:01:29 | 0:01:33 | |
Corrie! | 0:01:50 | 0:01:52 | |
-Come here. -Every owner will spend an average of £20,000 on their beloved dog in its lifetime. -Good boy. Sit. | 0:01:52 | 0:01:59 | |
We treat them as if they are fellow human beings | 0:01:59 | 0:02:03 | |
with all the thoughts, feelings and emotions of a family member. | 0:02:03 | 0:02:06 | |
-Good girl. -It's an incredibly close relationship. | 0:02:06 | 0:02:10 | |
We share our lives, our homes, even our beds with them. | 0:02:10 | 0:02:15 | |
-We're very close. We're best friends. -Pippin sleeps with us. | 0:02:15 | 0:02:18 | |
He loves being in the bed with his head on the pillow. | 0:02:18 | 0:02:22 | |
He just seems to fit in with | 0:02:22 | 0:02:25 | |
-your lifestyle. -She's there with my slippers first thing in the morning. | 0:02:25 | 0:02:29 | |
She's part of the family, she IS the family. | 0:02:29 | 0:02:32 | |
For decades, science has dismissed dogs as being unworthy of legitimate study. | 0:02:39 | 0:02:45 | |
But all that has changed. | 0:02:48 | 0:02:50 | |
Scientists are now attempting to understand dogs like never before. | 0:02:50 | 0:02:55 | |
How deep is the bond between us? | 0:02:55 | 0:02:59 | |
Where did this relationship come from? And ultimately, why is it dogs that are man's best friend? | 0:02:59 | 0:03:06 | |
Dogs are all over the world, they're everywhere. | 0:03:06 | 0:03:08 | |
Anywhere you find humans, you will almost certainly find dogs. | 0:03:08 | 0:03:11 | |
We're now beginning to realise that we can answer certain questions | 0:03:11 | 0:03:15 | |
in dogs that we can't really answer in any other species. | 0:03:15 | 0:03:17 | |
There's been this explosion in dog research, I think, | 0:03:17 | 0:03:20 | |
because they are specially tuned into humans and this makes dogs extremely interesting as a model. | 0:03:20 | 0:03:27 | |
Here at the University of Lincoln, Professor Daniel Mills is fascinated by dogs. | 0:03:34 | 0:03:40 | |
Using state-of-the-art technology, he wants to find evidence of how close our relationship really is. | 0:03:40 | 0:03:47 | |
What we're trying to do here is see the world from a dog's perspective rather than just impose our own | 0:03:47 | 0:03:53 | |
views as to how we think the dog sees the world. | 0:03:53 | 0:03:56 | |
He's attempting to discover if dogs are as good at reading our emotions as their owners claim. | 0:03:56 | 0:04:02 | |
He'll know what I'm thinking even before it's turned into a thought bubble. | 0:04:03 | 0:04:07 | |
He is clearly an animal, I accept that he is totally an animal. | 0:04:07 | 0:04:12 | |
I am not under any illusions that he isn't but he's more knowing than I would expect an animal to be. | 0:04:12 | 0:04:18 | |
He will look at me with sorrowful eyes and then give me one big lick on the hand as if to say it's all right. | 0:04:18 | 0:04:24 | |
It's this sixth sense that dogs have. | 0:04:24 | 0:04:26 | |
One of the things that a lot of people comment on | 0:04:29 | 0:04:33 | |
is that dogs seem to be naturally attuned to them and be able to sense their moods and whatever. | 0:04:33 | 0:04:37 | |
Part of our work here is to look into the scientific basis of that. | 0:04:37 | 0:04:41 | |
The key to a dog's ability to read our emotions might lie in something we all do without knowing it. | 0:04:43 | 0:04:51 | |
When we express our emotions in our faces, we don't do it symmetrically. | 0:04:51 | 0:04:55 | |
It's been shown that if you take somebody's face when they're expressing some emotion like | 0:04:55 | 0:04:59 | |
happiness or anger or something like that, there is a difference between the left and right side. | 0:04:59 | 0:05:05 | |
Composite faces consisting of two right or two left sides look very different. | 0:05:05 | 0:05:10 | |
One theory is maybe our emotions | 0:05:12 | 0:05:14 | |
are more faithfully presented in the right side of our face, and that's the side that we tune into. | 0:05:14 | 0:05:20 | |
When we look at a face, we have what's known as a natural | 0:05:20 | 0:05:22 | |
left gaze bias so you naturally look much more towards the left, ie the right-hand side of somebody's face. | 0:05:22 | 0:05:29 | |
Eye tracking software has demonstrated that | 0:05:31 | 0:05:34 | |
when presented with a human face, we nearly always look left first. | 0:05:34 | 0:05:39 | |
Daniel Mills wanted to find out if dogs used the same trick to read human faces. | 0:05:39 | 0:05:45 | |
Shifting the direction of your gaze we thought | 0:05:45 | 0:05:48 | |
thought was fairly unique to people until we started looking at dogs. | 0:05:48 | 0:05:51 | |
Tess, Tessy! | 0:05:51 | 0:05:55 | |
To test the theory, his team recreated this experiment with dogs. | 0:05:55 | 0:06:00 | |
-Bruce, what's that? -They presented a series of images showing human faces, | 0:06:00 | 0:06:05 | |
dog faces and inanimate objects and recorded the direction of a dog's gaze with a video camera. | 0:06:05 | 0:06:11 | |
We found that dogs when they are looking at pictures of dog faces | 0:06:11 | 0:06:18 | |
or objects, they will look randomly on the left or the right. | 0:06:18 | 0:06:22 | |
But when it came to human faces, they made a remarkable discovery. | 0:06:22 | 0:06:27 | |
So now we have Tess looking at a human face so first she's looking in the middle of the screen. | 0:06:28 | 0:06:34 | |
Here is the first eye movements on the left. | 0:06:34 | 0:06:38 | |
She's in the middle and she's going on the left, and then the dog is going to be even more on the left. | 0:06:38 | 0:06:45 | |
So now this is Moose and then we can see really well that this is a left gaze. | 0:06:45 | 0:06:52 | |
From here to here. | 0:06:52 | 0:06:54 | |
We can see the white here. | 0:06:54 | 0:06:57 | |
She's even moving her head. | 0:06:57 | 0:07:00 | |
As far as we know, no other animal has this relationship with the human face. | 0:07:04 | 0:07:10 | |
Dogs don't do this with each other. | 0:07:10 | 0:07:13 | |
Incredibly, it seems they've acquired a new skill to enable them to read our emotions. | 0:07:13 | 0:07:20 | |
Being able to detect when somebody is angry or potentially harmful, | 0:07:20 | 0:07:24 | |
you could understand that there may be a biological advantage in being able to read people's emotions. | 0:07:24 | 0:07:28 | |
Equally, it makes sense for a dog to approach somebody when they're smiling. | 0:07:28 | 0:07:32 | |
If dogs can read human emotion, and increasingly the scientific evidence is beginning | 0:07:34 | 0:07:39 | |
to point in that direction, that's going to form the basis of a very powerful bond between human and dog. | 0:07:39 | 0:07:45 | |
Evidence like this appears to underpin our conviction | 0:07:45 | 0:07:48 | |
that dogs understand us in a way that other animals cannot. | 0:07:48 | 0:07:52 | |
But for many dog owners, this unique relationship is much more than a one-way street. | 0:07:53 | 0:08:00 | |
I like to think we understand him. | 0:08:00 | 0:08:02 | |
Yes, but he woofs and we talk. | 0:08:02 | 0:08:05 | |
That's because he wants to be part of the conversation. | 0:08:05 | 0:08:08 | |
If he's bored, he'll take a deep sigh and go... | 0:08:08 | 0:08:11 | |
SHE WHINES LIKE DOG | 0:08:11 | 0:08:13 | |
He's got a bark when he wants to go out and he's got a bark... | 0:08:13 | 0:08:15 | |
SHE WOOFS | 0:08:15 | 0:08:18 | |
And a bark when he hears strange noises. | 0:08:18 | 0:08:21 | |
Sometimes when he tells the kittens off, he goes... | 0:08:21 | 0:08:24 | |
like that. SHE WOOFS | 0:08:24 | 0:08:26 | |
If you're in a certain mindset, you can almost understand what they're thinking. | 0:08:26 | 0:08:30 | |
The idea that we can understand barking almost like a language has always been dismissed by scientists. | 0:08:34 | 0:08:41 | |
But in Hungary, they are trying to see if there's any evidence to back up the claims made by dog owners. | 0:08:41 | 0:08:47 | |
Here, at Eotvos Lorand University in Budapest, | 0:08:47 | 0:08:49 | |
is the world's first research facility dedicated to investigating the human/dog relationship. | 0:08:49 | 0:08:57 | |
Dr Adam Miklosi wants to see if we humans really can understand dogs' barks. | 0:08:57 | 0:09:02 | |
Today, he's out on a field expedition collecting recordings. | 0:09:04 | 0:09:09 | |
Scientists used to think that barking is a random noise without any specific information or content. | 0:09:10 | 0:09:18 | |
However, we have a different idea. | 0:09:18 | 0:09:20 | |
Dogs might tell us something about their emotions, anger, fear, happiness, despair. | 0:09:20 | 0:09:27 | |
These are basic emotions which I think humans might be able to recognise in the barking sound. | 0:09:27 | 0:09:34 | |
To test this idea, Adam and his team acted out | 0:09:34 | 0:09:37 | |
a number of scenarios, provoking dogs to bark in different ways. | 0:09:37 | 0:09:42 | |
But when the recordings are played back to people, will they be able to match the bark to the emotion? | 0:09:42 | 0:09:49 | |
Alone bark. | 0:09:49 | 0:09:51 | |
THE DOG BARKS | 0:10:02 | 0:10:04 | |
That sounds like a dog asking for attention. | 0:10:04 | 0:10:05 | |
-It's anxious. -It's sad. | 0:10:08 | 0:10:11 | |
Distressed. | 0:10:11 | 0:10:13 | |
Want to be let off a chain or something like that. | 0:10:13 | 0:10:17 | |
THE DOG BARKS | 0:10:17 | 0:10:22 | |
-I think that one's playful. -Excitement. | 0:10:22 | 0:10:26 | |
It seems as though they're actually asking their owner for something. | 0:10:27 | 0:10:32 | |
It sounds as if it may want a ball or a toy or something. | 0:10:32 | 0:10:36 | |
She could be playing with it. | 0:10:36 | 0:10:38 | |
THE DOG BARKS | 0:10:41 | 0:10:43 | |
Angry. | 0:10:48 | 0:10:49 | |
This is a sound that she would make if she saw somebody behind the fence walking along. | 0:10:51 | 0:10:56 | |
it's a stranger encroaching on territory. | 0:10:58 | 0:11:01 | |
The results of Miklosi's research are remarkable. | 0:11:05 | 0:11:08 | |
It's proved there's incredibly strong agreement between people about what different barks mean. | 0:11:08 | 0:11:14 | |
'Overall in the study, you could see that people can discriminate' | 0:11:14 | 0:11:18 | |
six barks and most of them were quite successful in this. | 0:11:18 | 0:11:23 | |
Dr Miklosi has developed a system to analyse the barks. | 0:11:23 | 0:11:27 | |
It's helped him decode how dogs communicate meaning. | 0:11:27 | 0:11:30 | |
I measure the three features of this sound. | 0:11:32 | 0:11:35 | |
One was the frequency, the other was the tonality and the third was the interval between the barking sounds. | 0:11:35 | 0:11:42 | |
Probably, this is also what the judgment of people is | 0:11:42 | 0:11:46 | |
based when they are describing the bark in terms of emotional content. | 0:11:46 | 0:11:50 | |
But what's more surprising is not our ability to interpret the barks, but what it reveals about dogs. | 0:11:52 | 0:11:59 | |
In the natural world, dogs' wild relatives don't really bark. | 0:11:59 | 0:12:04 | |
Amazingly, it seems that during the course of domestication, dogs may have evolved | 0:12:04 | 0:12:09 | |
their elaborate vocal repertoire especially to communicate with us. | 0:12:09 | 0:12:14 | |
'At the basic level, everyone can do it and there is a good chance that' | 0:12:14 | 0:12:18 | |
barking is a very good means to communicate with humans. | 0:12:18 | 0:12:22 | |
The evidence from these recent experiments seems to confirm what dog owners have asserted all along, | 0:12:25 | 0:12:30 | |
that we're incredibly attuned to each other in a way that no other two species are. | 0:12:30 | 0:12:35 | |
But new research has uncovered that the bond between humans and dogs may be even deeper. | 0:12:38 | 0:12:44 | |
Research has turned to the most powerful bond, that between mother and baby, for clues. | 0:12:51 | 0:12:57 | |
It's really hard to describe. | 0:13:06 | 0:13:08 | |
It's just an amazing feeling. | 0:13:08 | 0:13:11 | |
In Sweden, Professor Kerstin Uvnas-Moberg has been studying the role | 0:13:11 | 0:13:16 | |
of the hormone oxytocin in bonding mothers with their newborn babies. | 0:13:16 | 0:13:20 | |
Oxytocin is a little, little heptide hormone. | 0:13:23 | 0:13:27 | |
It's just nine amino acids. | 0:13:27 | 0:13:29 | |
It's produced in a very old part of the brain called the hypothalamus | 0:13:29 | 0:13:34 | |
and oxytocin helps the mother quickly establish the positive feelings and the bond to the baby. | 0:13:34 | 0:13:42 | |
Each time a mother breast feeds, she has a new release of oxytocin and this reinforces the bond. | 0:13:42 | 0:13:49 | |
It's sort of in a way | 0:13:51 | 0:13:53 | |
difficult to understand how you can be familiar with somebody | 0:13:53 | 0:13:58 | |
-who is actually a stranger so quickly, don't you think? -Yes. | 0:13:58 | 0:14:02 | |
Professor Uvnas-Moberg believes oxytocin plays a similar role in the bond between dogs and their owners. | 0:14:10 | 0:14:18 | |
A lot of people would say, "Oh, it's not possible, dogs and humans, we're not the same. | 0:14:20 | 0:14:22 | |
"dogs and humans, it's very, very different." | 0:14:22 | 0:14:26 | |
But I would say that people who have dogs, who are used to animals and used to interaction with animals, | 0:14:26 | 0:14:32 | |
they would say, "Oh, that's not so strange." | 0:14:32 | 0:14:35 | |
To test the theory, blood samples were taken | 0:14:35 | 0:14:39 | |
from dogs and their owners before and during a petting session. | 0:14:39 | 0:14:44 | |
'We had a basal blood sample and there was nothing and then we | 0:14:45 | 0:14:48 | |
'had the sample taken at one minute and three minutes.' | 0:14:48 | 0:14:52 | |
You could see this beautiful peak of oxytocin. | 0:14:52 | 0:14:56 | |
'The fascinating thing is that the peak of oxytocin is similar' | 0:14:56 | 0:15:00 | |
to the one we see in breast feeding mothers. | 0:15:00 | 0:15:03 | |
Surprisingly, it's not just the owners who are affected. | 0:15:03 | 0:15:07 | |
Blood samples taken from dogs reveal a similar burst of oxytocin. | 0:15:07 | 0:15:13 | |
It is a mutual kind of interaction, you know. | 0:15:13 | 0:15:16 | |
The owner touches with her hands and they both smell, | 0:15:16 | 0:15:20 | |
hear and see each other. That is a very nice way of triggering oxytocin release in the two of them. | 0:15:20 | 0:15:28 | |
Oxytocin has a powerful physiological effect. | 0:15:31 | 0:15:33 | |
It lowers the heart rate and blood pressure, leading to reduced levels of stress. | 0:15:33 | 0:15:39 | |
Research indicates that owning a dog could even extend your life. | 0:15:42 | 0:15:47 | |
If you have a dog, | 0:15:48 | 0:15:50 | |
you are much less likely to have a heart attack and if you have a heart attack, you are three to four | 0:15:50 | 0:15:56 | |
times more likely to survive it if you have a dog than if you don't. | 0:15:56 | 0:16:01 | |
So where does this incredible relationship come from? | 0:16:16 | 0:16:19 | |
When did it start and how? | 0:16:19 | 0:16:22 | |
It's a question that has puzzled scientists ever since Darwin. | 0:16:22 | 0:16:26 | |
He recognised the special relationship we have with dogs but was at a loss to explain it. | 0:16:26 | 0:16:32 | |
Darwin couldn't even say for sure which animal was the true ancestor of the dog. | 0:16:32 | 0:16:38 | |
It's a complex puzzle that both archaeologists | 0:16:38 | 0:16:42 | |
and molecular geneticists have been working to solve. | 0:16:42 | 0:16:46 | |
There's a huge amount of variation in present-day dogs. | 0:16:46 | 0:16:48 | |
Consider the difference between a Pekinese and a Great Dane. | 0:16:48 | 0:16:53 | |
Could they really all be descended from one wild ancestor? | 0:16:53 | 0:16:59 | |
It could have been a coyote that might have intergressed with a wolf and then that may | 0:16:59 | 0:17:04 | |
have been slightly selected upon to create one particular breed of dog, or jackals or African wild dogs. | 0:17:04 | 0:17:11 | |
Any number of these other dog-like species that are out there | 0:17:11 | 0:17:13 | |
must have come together and that's where that variation must have come. | 0:17:13 | 0:17:18 | |
Until the advent of molecular genetics, archaeology had few firm answers. | 0:17:18 | 0:17:24 | |
All you have to play with are the bones and so when you look at the bones, if you don't have | 0:17:24 | 0:17:29 | |
a very small flat-faced round-headed pug in the archaeological record, you don't know where that came from. | 0:17:29 | 0:17:36 | |
Those are questions that before genetics you really couldn't answer. | 0:17:36 | 0:17:39 | |
To unravel the evolutionary origins of dogs, molecular | 0:17:40 | 0:17:43 | |
geneticists compared DNA from dogs with that of their wild relatives. | 0:17:43 | 0:17:49 | |
Specifically, they looked at mitochondrial DNA sequences | 0:17:50 | 0:17:54 | |
which are passed unchanged down the maternal line. | 0:17:54 | 0:17:58 | |
What's so useful for scientists is that mitochondrial DNA changes little over time | 0:17:58 | 0:18:04 | |
and so acts as a kind of signature left by an animal's ancestors. | 0:18:04 | 0:18:09 | |
Those markers in domestic dogs show them to be much more | 0:18:10 | 0:18:13 | |
closely related to grey wolves than they are to any other species. | 0:18:13 | 0:18:17 | |
There's no admixture so we never see a mitochondrial signature of | 0:18:17 | 0:18:20 | |
an African wild dog, jackal or coyote in a domestic dog. | 0:18:20 | 0:18:24 | |
Thousands upon thousands of mitochondrial DNA that has been | 0:18:24 | 0:18:27 | |
extracted from domestic dogs, every single one of them just looks just like a grey wolf. | 0:18:27 | 0:18:31 | |
It's now without doubt that dogs are domesticated wolves, | 0:18:33 | 0:18:38 | |
but how and when did it happen? | 0:18:38 | 0:18:41 | |
-Again, the archaeological record is inconclusive. -What is clearly a dog? | 0:18:41 | 0:18:46 | |
Clearly, a dog is something which is clearly not a wolf. | 0:18:46 | 0:18:50 | |
Well, here's a wolf skull and as you can see it's a long, quite low skull with a relatively flat top. | 0:18:50 | 0:18:59 | |
The teeth are quite large and the thing is quite narrow. | 0:18:59 | 0:19:02 | |
Compare that with a domestic dog. | 0:19:02 | 0:19:05 | |
This is a cairn terrier and as you can see, | 0:19:05 | 0:19:08 | |
the process of domestication has gone really quite a long way. | 0:19:08 | 0:19:11 | |
The whole face is very much shorter, it's been contracted towards the brain case. | 0:19:11 | 0:19:16 | |
The brain case itself has a much steeper front and a much more bowed upper surface. | 0:19:16 | 0:19:21 | |
If you found that, you would be in no doubt you were dealing with a domestic dog. | 0:19:21 | 0:19:24 | |
But this is a domestic Alsatian and telling these apart really would be substantially difficult. | 0:19:24 | 0:19:31 | |
And since early dogs were probably very wolf-like, it's hard to pinpoint | 0:19:32 | 0:19:37 | |
when domestication happened by looking at the shape of the bones. | 0:19:37 | 0:19:42 | |
The best I can give you is around 12 or 13,000 years ago. | 0:19:42 | 0:19:47 | |
We start seeing the first things that everybody would accept as being domestic dogs. | 0:19:47 | 0:19:51 | |
But mitochondrial DNA offered a different set of clues. | 0:19:51 | 0:19:55 | |
The original genetic data that were coming out seemed to suggest that domestication was happening on | 0:19:55 | 0:19:59 | |
a far earlier timescale than was suggested by anything in the archaeological record. | 0:19:59 | 0:20:04 | |
The first dates that were coming out were on the order of 100,000 | 0:20:04 | 0:20:08 | |
years or more, which a lot of archaeologists raised their eyebrows at. | 0:20:08 | 0:20:13 | |
It's hotly debated exactly when dogs were domesticated, | 0:20:13 | 0:20:17 | |
but there's one thing archaeologists and geneticists agree on: | 0:20:17 | 0:20:21 | |
our relationship with dogs goes back thousands of years further than with any other pet. | 0:20:21 | 0:20:27 | |
It was a time when we were still hunter-gatherers. | 0:20:30 | 0:20:34 | |
Dogs were certainly the first animal to be domesticated, and they fit into hunting and | 0:20:35 | 0:20:41 | |
gathering societies probably better than any other species out there. | 0:20:41 | 0:20:46 | |
At this stage when we're hunting and gathering and killing wild animals, | 0:20:46 | 0:20:50 | |
after you finish with them you're creating a relatively large pile | 0:20:50 | 0:20:54 | |
of bone and leftover meat, things that these wolves would have been very attracted to. | 0:20:54 | 0:20:58 | |
Those wolves that were able to take advantage of that resource, and were a little bit less afraid and could | 0:20:58 | 0:21:04 | |
approach the human camp, were then setting themselves up into a closer relationship with humans. | 0:21:04 | 0:21:09 | |
We are carnivores, | 0:21:11 | 0:21:12 | |
we are social carnivores, we hunt in groups and we hunt in daylight. | 0:21:12 | 0:21:18 | |
There are not many other species that do that. | 0:21:18 | 0:21:21 | |
The wolf is a social carnivore that hunts by daylight, and therefore, | 0:21:21 | 0:21:26 | |
there's natural potential for teamwork between those two species. | 0:21:26 | 0:21:30 | |
We became much better hunters with dogs. | 0:21:34 | 0:21:37 | |
We are more successfully taking down large game, which means we have more food to eat, which means we can have | 0:21:37 | 0:21:42 | |
more offspring, which means the overall populations of humans grow. | 0:21:42 | 0:21:45 | |
Dog domestication may have helped pave the way for a fundamental change in human lifestyle. | 0:21:48 | 0:21:55 | |
It's hard to see how early herders would have moved and protected | 0:21:55 | 0:22:01 | |
and guarded their flocks without domestic dogs being in place. | 0:22:01 | 0:22:05 | |
And one has to wonder whether agriculture would ever really have | 0:22:05 | 0:22:08 | |
made it as a viable alternative to hunting and gathering. | 0:22:08 | 0:22:12 | |
Some believe that the influence of dogs on our development was not just important, but pivotal. | 0:22:15 | 0:22:21 | |
Dogs absolutely turn the tables. | 0:22:21 | 0:22:23 | |
Without dogs, humans would still be hunter gatherers. | 0:22:23 | 0:22:27 | |
And without that initial starting phase of dog domestication, | 0:22:27 | 0:22:30 | |
civilisation would not have been possible. | 0:22:30 | 0:22:32 | |
We look at our dogs and we see an intelligence, | 0:22:49 | 0:22:53 | |
an ability to interact with us unlike any other domesticated animal. | 0:22:53 | 0:22:58 | |
But are dogs really that clever, or are they just dumb animals taught | 0:22:58 | 0:23:02 | |
to perform tricks that mimic human behaviour? | 0:23:02 | 0:23:06 | |
I think she's very smart. She learns tricks fairly quickly. | 0:23:06 | 0:23:10 | |
If I am packing a suitcase, they will go and sit in | 0:23:10 | 0:23:13 | |
the suitcase because they know that suitcase is going to go somewhere. | 0:23:13 | 0:23:17 | |
When I'm talking to him most of the time, | 0:23:17 | 0:23:20 | |
his little head usually jilts to the side as if he knows what I'm saying. | 0:23:20 | 0:23:25 | |
I do talk to her and she picks up on what I say to her. | 0:23:25 | 0:23:29 | |
I know it sounds stupid, but I do have a conversation with my dog. | 0:23:29 | 0:23:32 | |
But how does the intelligence of a dog really compare in the animal kingdom? | 0:23:38 | 0:23:42 | |
New research is discovering that in certain ways, dogs may actually think | 0:23:42 | 0:23:47 | |
more like us than any other animal, including our nearest relative, the chimpanzee. | 0:23:47 | 0:23:53 | |
Of all the questions around the evolution of human cognition, | 0:23:55 | 0:23:58 | |
of course, people would focus in on chimps quite naturally. | 0:23:58 | 0:24:01 | |
Suddenly, there were dogs doing something not even chimps could do. | 0:24:01 | 0:24:05 | |
Cognitive psychologist Juliane Kaminski from | 0:24:06 | 0:24:09 | |
the Max Planck Institute in Germany, has been comparing chimps with dogs in a series of revealing experiments. | 0:24:09 | 0:24:17 | |
At Leipzig Zoo, Juliane is testing chimps to see if they can understand | 0:24:26 | 0:24:30 | |
human gestures, like pointing, to find a hidden treat. | 0:24:30 | 0:24:35 | |
As simple as it seems to us, even our nearest primate relatives failed the task miserably. | 0:24:44 | 0:24:50 | |
She's not really focusing on me and she's simply making her own choice. | 0:24:50 | 0:24:55 | |
Most of the time you can see that she makes a decision long before I give my gesture. | 0:24:55 | 0:25:00 | |
She doesn't even wait for my information. | 0:25:00 | 0:25:03 | |
It's such an uncooperative attraction, so it's like really I'm providing information for her to | 0:25:03 | 0:25:09 | |
find food, which is just simply something which would never happen in a chimp group, really. | 0:25:09 | 0:25:15 | |
A chimp wouldn't go like, "Oh, look there's the banana", | 0:25:15 | 0:25:17 | |
and then another chimp could go and get it. | 0:25:17 | 0:25:19 | |
Since we're the only species that makes this gesture, | 0:25:24 | 0:25:27 | |
it would be remarkable if any animal could understand it. | 0:25:27 | 0:25:30 | |
But dog owners take it for granted that their dogs respond to pointing. | 0:25:34 | 0:25:39 | |
Good boy! | 0:25:39 | 0:25:40 | |
For Kaminski, it's proof of their extraordinary social intelligence. | 0:25:40 | 0:25:46 | |
If you really look at that gesture, it's an informative gesture. | 0:25:46 | 0:25:50 | |
So it's in its essence a very cooperative | 0:25:50 | 0:25:52 | |
interaction, so I'm really helping you to find something. | 0:25:52 | 0:25:55 | |
And for dogs, following, pointing seems to be very natural, and it makes dogs extremely interesting. | 0:25:55 | 0:26:02 | |
In fact, dogs are so tuned into our social cues, | 0:26:02 | 0:26:06 | |
they can even pick up on something as subtle as the direction of our gaze. | 0:26:06 | 0:26:10 | |
Humans have unique almond-shaped eyes with exposed white sclera visible on each side. | 0:26:18 | 0:26:24 | |
One hypothesis is that we have evolved those eyes because we use it for communication. | 0:26:24 | 0:26:30 | |
With human eyes you can really tell easily which direction I'm looking. | 0:26:30 | 0:26:35 | |
We think that maybe dogs are really tuned into that, | 0:26:35 | 0:26:38 | |
and really are interested in human eyes because of that. | 0:26:38 | 0:26:41 | |
But these aren't skills that dogs use with each other. | 0:26:41 | 0:26:45 | |
They are abilities dogs only use with humans. | 0:26:45 | 0:26:49 | |
I think it's very, very easy to imagine that they develop special skills in | 0:26:49 | 0:26:54 | |
interacting with humans, because that's their new social partner. | 0:26:54 | 0:26:57 | |
So they kind of learn to interpret human communication, which is different from dog communication. | 0:26:57 | 0:27:03 | |
So they kind of learned a second language, so you could probably say they are bilingual, yes. | 0:27:03 | 0:27:08 | |
Even puppies as young as six weeks old seem to intelligently respond to human gestures. | 0:27:12 | 0:27:18 | |
At least some of the time! | 0:27:20 | 0:27:23 | |
The fact they're quite young puppies can do something, | 0:27:25 | 0:27:29 | |
if they learn it, they learn it very quickly, and it's obviously that | 0:27:29 | 0:27:33 | |
they are ready to do it - so from the very beginning they are ready to receive human communication. | 0:27:33 | 0:27:40 | |
As dog owners, we think we understand the limits of our dogs' intelligence. | 0:27:44 | 0:27:48 | |
But now some dogs are challenging our assumptions. | 0:27:48 | 0:27:52 | |
We may have to reconsider how clever dogs are. | 0:27:52 | 0:27:55 | |
Juliane Kaminski has discovered a remarkable dog living in Austria, just outside Vienna. | 0:27:55 | 0:28:01 | |
She's conducted a series of experiments, and is amazed at the dog's intelligence. | 0:28:01 | 0:28:07 | |
Known only the pseudonym Betsy, the true identity of this | 0:28:07 | 0:28:11 | |
seven-year-old border collie is a closely guarded secret. | 0:28:11 | 0:28:14 | |
She can distinguish objects by name, | 0:28:14 | 0:28:16 | |
which is really amazing, and she has many, many words. | 0:28:16 | 0:28:20 | |
Kase. | 0:28:20 | 0:28:22 | |
Das Zebra. | 0:28:24 | 0:28:27 | |
With a vocabulary of over 340 words, Betsy is pushing the boundaries of what we think dogs are capable of. | 0:28:27 | 0:28:34 | |
Karotte. | 0:28:37 | 0:28:39 | |
Sandwich. | 0:28:39 | 0:28:42 | |
I think it was when she was four or five months old, | 0:28:48 | 0:28:53 | |
when she spontaneously started to connect human words to items. | 0:28:53 | 0:28:57 | |
When we were discussing shall we play with the rope, | 0:28:57 | 0:29:00 | |
or with the ball, she immediately started to bring those items. So it was actually her idea. | 0:29:00 | 0:29:05 | |
And from this time on we started to really train her on different words. | 0:29:05 | 0:29:10 | |
It was maybe one toy per week, and it worked. | 0:29:10 | 0:29:13 | |
I think on average a well-trained dog maybe knows like 15 commands or something. | 0:29:15 | 0:29:19 | |
There are just very few individuals who can do what she does. | 0:29:19 | 0:29:23 | |
I can tell that I tried it with my own dog and it didn't work at all. | 0:29:23 | 0:29:26 | |
So he could maybe distinguish two objects after a while and after extensive training, | 0:29:26 | 0:29:31 | |
but she is really able to learn this easily and more than 300 objects, that's pretty amazing. | 0:29:31 | 0:29:37 | |
Betsy's understanding of vocabulary rivals that of a two year old, | 0:29:37 | 0:29:42 | |
so Kaminski decided to test her on other key developmental milestones. | 0:29:42 | 0:29:47 | |
Can you go find me one of them over there? Yeah? | 0:29:47 | 0:29:50 | |
Two year olds are just beginning to understand | 0:29:50 | 0:29:53 | |
the use of physical symbols, such as scale models in communication. | 0:29:53 | 0:29:58 | |
Though it looks easy, it requires abstract thinking way beyond the capability of almost all animals. | 0:29:58 | 0:30:06 | |
But would Betsy be able to do this too? | 0:30:06 | 0:30:09 | |
SHE SPEAKS GERMAN | 0:30:18 | 0:30:19 | |
This was something the owners have never tried before, so when I came and I said, "I want to do this," | 0:30:24 | 0:30:28 | |
they were like, "No way, that's not going to work", but I was the first one doing it with her. | 0:30:28 | 0:30:33 | |
And she had no problem doing it right from the beginning. | 0:30:33 | 0:30:36 | |
This is surprising because in its essence if I hold out an object, | 0:30:36 | 0:30:40 | |
she turns it into something communicative, | 0:30:40 | 0:30:42 | |
and that's so interesting. | 0:30:42 | 0:30:45 | |
-What about this one? -Children also begin to grasp that a drawing or photograph can depict a real object. | 0:30:49 | 0:30:56 | |
-Thank you very much, well done. -No other dog has ever achieved this under trial conditions. | 0:30:56 | 0:31:00 | |
But once again, Betsy picked this up almost immediately. | 0:31:02 | 0:31:06 | |
In its essence the picture is something very different as the object, so it's a piece of paper | 0:31:11 | 0:31:19 | |
and it's two-dimensional, but it's representing something, so she obviously interprets | 0:31:19 | 0:31:24 | |
that as representing an object, a three-dimensional object, and that's so interesting that she does this. | 0:31:24 | 0:31:30 | |
I know exactly what you want. | 0:31:30 | 0:31:32 | |
This is the one you want and I'm going to go and get it for you. | 0:31:32 | 0:31:36 | |
SHE SPEAKS GERMAN | 0:31:36 | 0:31:40 | |
Kaminski is unsure how many dogs might have similar abilities, but Betsy is proof | 0:31:42 | 0:31:47 | |
that certain dogs may have the potential to be more intelligent than we ever thought possible. | 0:31:47 | 0:31:52 | |
So how did the dog acquire these unique abilities? | 0:32:00 | 0:32:03 | |
Did they evolve them over thousands of years, | 0:32:03 | 0:32:06 | |
or is it the way dogs have been brought up in a human environment that counts? | 0:32:06 | 0:32:10 | |
Dogs and wolves are still the same species today. | 0:32:15 | 0:32:18 | |
They can easily interbreed. | 0:32:18 | 0:32:20 | |
Overall, wolves and dogs are 99.8% genetically identical. | 0:32:20 | 0:32:25 | |
Given they're biologically so similar, is it the way we raise them in our homes that makes a dog? | 0:32:28 | 0:32:34 | |
Scientists in Hungary set out to answer this question. | 0:32:34 | 0:32:39 | |
-We wanted to see whether the special relationships between humans | 0:32:39 | 0:32:45 | |
and dogs are due to nature or nurture. | 0:32:45 | 0:32:48 | |
So we wanted to see what happens if a wolf is raised | 0:32:48 | 0:32:53 | |
in a human environment, in a home, whether it would act like a dog or not. | 0:32:53 | 0:32:59 | |
A litter of five-day old wolf cubs was taken from a wolf sanctuary outside Budapest. | 0:33:01 | 0:33:07 | |
A group of young researchers became their adoptive parents, caring for them 24 hours a day. | 0:33:07 | 0:33:14 | |
As a control for the experiment, they'd already raised puppies. | 0:33:14 | 0:33:17 | |
Now they aimed to raise the wolf cubs the same way. | 0:33:17 | 0:33:22 | |
So we were especially nice with our cubs, because we wanted to maintain a very good relationship with them. | 0:33:22 | 0:33:29 | |
They were really cute, so it was not very difficult to carry them everywhere we were going. | 0:33:32 | 0:33:39 | |
And we also slept together with the cubs. | 0:33:39 | 0:33:43 | |
So the bonding, it was good. | 0:33:43 | 0:33:46 | |
I really liked my cubs and there was a really strong relationship between us. | 0:33:46 | 0:33:52 | |
But then something began to change. | 0:33:52 | 0:33:56 | |
Despite raising the cubs in the same way as the puppies, by eight weeks the differences had started to show. | 0:33:56 | 0:34:02 | |
Dog puppies were always interested in what I was doing. | 0:34:05 | 0:34:10 | |
There is a very strong co-operative | 0:34:10 | 0:34:13 | |
tendency in dogs and this was missing in wolves. | 0:34:13 | 0:34:19 | |
They had their own ideas, they were not much interested in my activities. | 0:34:19 | 0:34:25 | |
The researchers wanted to find out what was going on, and decided to run a series of tests | 0:34:27 | 0:34:33 | |
comparing the wolf cubs with puppies of the same age. | 0:34:33 | 0:34:37 | |
TOY YAPS | 0:34:37 | 0:34:39 | |
Unlike dogs, the wolf cubs didn't respond to pointing. | 0:34:55 | 0:34:59 | |
In fact they hardly made eye contact with humans at all. | 0:34:59 | 0:35:02 | |
The cubs were behaving as they would do in the wild. | 0:35:04 | 0:35:07 | |
CUB GROWLS | 0:35:10 | 0:35:12 | |
She was really possessive. | 0:35:15 | 0:35:17 | |
If she wanted to grab an object, it was really difficult to get it back. | 0:35:17 | 0:35:22 | |
And if we wanted to open the refrigerator and have breakfast, | 0:35:22 | 0:35:29 | |
the pup was immediately in the middle of the refrigerator and grabbed something. | 0:35:29 | 0:35:33 | |
It is not like with a dog that you say, "No, you shouldn't." | 0:35:33 | 0:35:39 | |
It just didn't care. | 0:35:39 | 0:35:40 | |
The battles continued to get worse. | 0:35:45 | 0:35:47 | |
After the second month, we started to have more and more conflicts | 0:35:47 | 0:35:53 | |
and the wolves wanted to destroy everything. | 0:35:53 | 0:35:56 | |
And of course when the cub is a small cub, | 0:35:57 | 0:36:00 | |
it's nothing, but when they reach 40 or 50 kg, you know, | 0:36:00 | 0:36:06 | |
it starts to be really dangerous. | 0:36:06 | 0:36:08 | |
We just could not keep them in the house any more. | 0:36:08 | 0:36:13 | |
Hoo! | 0:36:13 | 0:36:16 | |
After four months the cubs had to be returned to the reserve. | 0:36:16 | 0:36:20 | |
The experiment had proved that upbringing has little impact. | 0:36:20 | 0:36:24 | |
It's impossible to turn a wolf into a dog, no matter how much you nurture it. | 0:36:24 | 0:36:29 | |
So according to our experiences, the dog is not a socialised wolf at all. | 0:36:31 | 0:36:37 | |
These differences we experienced in the community viability | 0:36:37 | 0:36:42 | |
and in the social behaviour of dogs, this is the effect of domestication. | 0:36:42 | 0:36:47 | |
The difference must lie in the way dogs have been bred by humans over thousands of years. | 0:36:47 | 0:36:53 | |
Their unique abilities are now part of their nature. | 0:36:53 | 0:36:58 | |
But how did dogs evolve these innate attributes? | 0:37:03 | 0:37:07 | |
What was the process that made them intrinsically tame? | 0:37:07 | 0:37:11 | |
A remarkable experiment in Siberia may hold the key to understanding how wolves turned into dogs. | 0:37:14 | 0:37:22 | |
CAR HORN TOOTS | 0:37:22 | 0:37:24 | |
50 years ago, Soviet scientists set up a breeding programme to try and domesticate silver foxes. | 0:37:26 | 0:37:33 | |
The scale of the project has opened a remarkable window on domestication. | 0:37:37 | 0:37:42 | |
It's become a focal point for scientists across the world. | 0:37:42 | 0:37:45 | |
Here on a farm outside the city of Novosibirsk, | 0:37:47 | 0:37:52 | |
the experiment still continues today, overseen by Dr Lyudmila Trut. | 0:37:52 | 0:37:57 | |
The breeding programme began in 1959 when the first foxes were selected from local fur farms. | 0:37:59 | 0:38:06 | |
TRANSLATION: We approached the animals in the cages | 0:38:11 | 0:38:15 | |
and recorded their reactions to us. | 0:38:15 | 0:38:17 | |
We could see that some of the foxes showed aggressive behaviour. | 0:38:17 | 0:38:22 | |
Others were frightened. | 0:38:22 | 0:38:24 | |
But only 1% of them showed neither signs of fear nor aggression. | 0:38:24 | 0:38:29 | |
This 1% was selected to become the founding generation of a new population of foxes. | 0:38:33 | 0:38:40 | |
At every generation, the selection process was repeated with only the tamest foxes being allowed to breed. | 0:38:41 | 0:38:48 | |
Within just three generations, the aggressive behaviour began to disappear. | 0:38:48 | 0:38:54 | |
TRANSLATION: The radical changes came through in the eighth generation, | 0:38:55 | 0:39:01 | |
when foxes started to seek contact with humans | 0:39:01 | 0:39:04 | |
and show affection to them. | 0:39:04 | 0:39:06 | |
The amazing thing was that cubs who had just started to crawl, opened their eyes | 0:39:06 | 0:39:14 | |
and started showing affection to humans | 0:39:14 | 0:39:16 | |
while breathing heavily, wagging their tales and howling. | 0:39:16 | 0:39:21 | |
This kind of response was a big surprise to us. | 0:39:21 | 0:39:26 | |
Half a century on, the 50th generation of foxes are tamer than ever. | 0:39:30 | 0:39:35 | |
It's an accelerated model of how dogs might have been domesticated from wolves. | 0:39:38 | 0:39:43 | |
But tame foxes alone cannot unravel the mystery of domestication. | 0:39:50 | 0:39:56 | |
A parallel group of silver foxes have also been bred to retain their aggressive behaviour. | 0:39:56 | 0:40:01 | |
TRANSLATION: It just bit my hand. | 0:40:21 | 0:40:25 | |
TRANSLATION: I didn't even open the cage. | 0:40:27 | 0:40:31 | |
I just put my hand up and it managed to bite me through the bars. | 0:40:31 | 0:40:35 | |
This isn't a fox - it's a dragon. | 0:40:43 | 0:40:46 | |
It's allowed researchers to make unique comparisons between tame and aggressive foxes. | 0:40:48 | 0:40:54 | |
TRANSLATION: We did an experiment with cross-fostering | 0:40:58 | 0:41:02 | |
where we gave tame cubs to aggressive mothers. | 0:41:02 | 0:41:06 | |
and vice versa. We found out that the mother's behaviour does not influence that of the cub. | 0:41:06 | 0:41:12 | |
This cub was brought up by a tame mother. | 0:41:13 | 0:41:17 | |
It showed something remarkable, that the difference | 0:41:20 | 0:41:23 | |
between tame and aggressive foxes is largely in their genes. | 0:41:23 | 0:41:27 | |
TRANSLATION: We even took the experiment one stage further and transplanted embryos | 0:41:27 | 0:41:34 | |
from aggressive mothers into tame mothers, | 0:41:34 | 0:41:38 | |
but the results were the same. It proved that you can't change the gene of aggressiveness | 0:41:38 | 0:41:44 | |
and it will be kept and preserved for the next generation. | 0:41:44 | 0:41:47 | |
Geneticists have already located several genetic regions responsible for tameness. | 0:41:48 | 0:41:54 | |
They're now taking blood samples from tame and aggressive foxes | 0:41:54 | 0:41:58 | |
in an attempt to pinpoint the specific genes. | 0:41:58 | 0:42:00 | |
Dr Anna Kukekova, a molecular geneticist based at Cornell University in the USA, | 0:42:03 | 0:42:09 | |
has travelled over 5,000 miles to study the foxes. | 0:42:09 | 0:42:12 | |
Behaviour is complex. | 0:42:13 | 0:42:15 | |
We're pretty sure there will be not a single gene different | 0:42:17 | 0:42:20 | |
with the orchestra of genes | 0:42:20 | 0:42:22 | |
which is responsible for this behaviour. | 0:42:22 | 0:42:25 | |
She's hoping that once the precise genes are identified, it will lead | 0:42:27 | 0:42:31 | |
to a better understanding of the biology of tameness. | 0:42:31 | 0:42:34 | |
He is like a doggy, you know, like the puppy | 0:42:36 | 0:42:40 | |
who's very happy when somebody picks him up from the floor. | 0:42:40 | 0:42:44 | |
It's unbelievable how they trust, how they trust people and I just really admire these animals. | 0:42:47 | 0:42:54 | |
TRANSLATION: So within 50 years of our intensive selection process, | 0:43:08 | 0:43:12 | |
this fire-breathing dragon has turned into a human friend. | 0:43:12 | 0:43:16 | |
If foxes were brought up in a domestic environment, | 0:43:18 | 0:43:23 | |
interacting with other animals and humans, they would make fantastic pets. | 0:43:23 | 0:43:28 | |
They are as independent as cats, but, at the same time, as devoted as any dog could be. | 0:43:29 | 0:43:37 | |
But it's not just the fox's behaviour that has changed. | 0:43:51 | 0:43:56 | |
Just a few generations into the experiment, scientists began to notice a curious phenomenon. | 0:43:56 | 0:44:02 | |
The normal pattern and silver colour of the coat changed dramatically in some of the tame foxes. | 0:44:04 | 0:44:10 | |
Their tails often became curly instead of straight. | 0:44:10 | 0:44:14 | |
Some young foxes kept their floppy ears for much longer than usual, | 0:44:14 | 0:44:20 | |
and their limbs and tails generally became shorter than their wild counterparts. | 0:44:20 | 0:44:25 | |
In effect, the tame silver foxes were beginning to look more like dogs. | 0:44:25 | 0:44:31 | |
What this shows is that when you select against aggression, | 0:44:38 | 0:44:41 | |
you get almost all the same suite of changes that you see when you compare dogs to wolves. | 0:44:41 | 0:44:47 | |
American anthropologist, Professor Brian Hare has visited the breeding programme in Siberia. | 0:44:47 | 0:44:52 | |
He believes it shows that if you select for tameness, changes in appearance will naturally follow. | 0:44:52 | 0:44:59 | |
I think the surprise when thinking about dog origins is that | 0:45:02 | 0:45:05 | |
there's so many ways that dogs are different from wolves. | 0:45:05 | 0:45:09 | |
So is it that you had to select for each of these traits individually? | 0:45:09 | 0:45:12 | |
Well, the answer from the fox work is no. | 0:45:12 | 0:45:15 | |
If you just select for behaviour, a lot of the morphological and physiological changes | 0:45:15 | 0:45:20 | |
between wolves and dogs, get dragged along. | 0:45:20 | 0:45:22 | |
You end up with this crazy variance, you know | 0:45:22 | 0:45:25 | |
floppy ears, curly tails, you know, all these other things that are really cute to talk about. | 0:45:25 | 0:45:31 | |
So you get a lot of stuff for free when you select against aggression. | 0:45:31 | 0:45:35 | |
It's enabled him to draw some surprising conclusions about the process of domestication. | 0:45:35 | 0:45:40 | |
When you're selecting against aggression, what you're doing is you're favouring juvenile traits. | 0:45:42 | 0:45:48 | |
Juveniles and infants show much less aggression than adults | 0:45:48 | 0:45:52 | |
and so what the idea is, is that you know | 0:45:52 | 0:45:55 | |
basically you've frozen development at a much earlier stage | 0:45:55 | 0:46:01 | |
and so you have an animal as an adult that looks and behaves much more like a juvenile. | 0:46:01 | 0:46:06 | |
The theory is that dogs are in many ways like juvenile wolves. | 0:46:06 | 0:46:12 | |
It explains how dogs could have begun to look so different from the wolves they came from. | 0:46:12 | 0:46:16 | |
It's amazing that you get this variance | 0:46:18 | 0:46:20 | |
that's hidden under the surface that expresses itself. | 0:46:20 | 0:46:23 | |
And then later people can directly decide, "I really like the one | 0:46:23 | 0:46:27 | |
"with the curly tail and I'm going to put them together." | 0:46:27 | 0:46:30 | |
And then you can end up having dogs that you know sort of shift in ways that people want them to go. | 0:46:30 | 0:46:36 | |
In the past few hundred years, we've taken dogs' infantile features | 0:46:42 | 0:46:45 | |
and emphasised them even further through selective breeding. | 0:46:45 | 0:46:51 | |
We've created hundreds of breeds to fulfil different roles, | 0:46:51 | 0:46:55 | |
but some of them have been bred purely for their looks. | 0:46:55 | 0:46:58 | |
I think this kind of breeding really tells us a lot | 0:46:58 | 0:47:01 | |
about what kind of people we are, what it is that we like about dogs. | 0:47:01 | 0:47:06 | |
How would you to describe Laddy in one word. | 0:47:06 | 0:47:10 | |
Uh... | 0:47:10 | 0:47:12 | |
..cute. | 0:47:12 | 0:47:13 | |
Cute, yeah. | 0:47:13 | 0:47:15 | |
Cute, adorable and funny. | 0:47:15 | 0:47:19 | |
I just look at her and I just smile. | 0:47:19 | 0:47:21 | |
She's particularly cute when she's sleeping. | 0:47:21 | 0:47:24 | |
We all know we find them cute, but what is it exactly that makes us respond to dogs so powerfully? | 0:47:26 | 0:47:32 | |
Psychiatrist, Morton Kringlebach, | 0:47:34 | 0:47:37 | |
has a theory as to why the way dogs look has such a profound impact on us. | 0:47:37 | 0:47:42 | |
The need to nurture I think is something that is so deep in us | 0:47:42 | 0:47:45 | |
that we find it very difficult to resist. | 0:47:45 | 0:47:47 | |
Dogs, puppies have very infant-like features and maybe that's one of the reasons why we think | 0:47:49 | 0:47:57 | |
they are so cute is that they remind us of the infants that we are - so to speak - programmed to like. | 0:47:57 | 0:48:04 | |
There's something about the way that the facial features are organised that makes us want to care for them. | 0:48:04 | 0:48:11 | |
It's about having a large forehead, it's about having large eyes, big ears... | 0:48:11 | 0:48:15 | |
And there's something about that that almost unconsciously we cannot help ourselves but actually like. | 0:48:15 | 0:48:21 | |
Are you feeding him now? | 0:48:25 | 0:48:28 | |
We're just going to go one more scan. | 0:48:28 | 0:48:31 | |
Dr Kringlebach is interested in exploring how strongly we respond to these infantile features. | 0:48:31 | 0:48:38 | |
A state of the art MEG scanner was used to measure people's brain activity | 0:48:40 | 0:48:44 | |
while they were looking at images of baby faces and adult faces. | 0:48:44 | 0:48:48 | |
We found that within a seventh of a second there was activity in the frontal part of the brain, | 0:48:54 | 0:48:59 | |
just over the eyebrows, in the orbitofrontal of cortex that was present when you were looking | 0:48:59 | 0:49:06 | |
at the infant faces but not when you were looking at the adult faces. | 0:49:06 | 0:49:11 | |
This part of the brain is very much involved in emotional responses, and so what we think we may | 0:49:11 | 0:49:17 | |
have stumbled across here is really in many ways the brain equivalent of the parental instinct. | 0:49:17 | 0:49:23 | |
There's almost like a wired-in automatic reaction. | 0:49:23 | 0:49:28 | |
Kringlebach is now testing to see if we have a similar response to dogs' cute features. | 0:49:30 | 0:49:36 | |
The data is still being analysed but he suspects | 0:49:36 | 0:49:39 | |
there will be a comparable signature in regions of the brain associated with nurturing responses. | 0:49:39 | 0:49:45 | |
Just as with the infant, when you're looking at dogs, | 0:49:48 | 0:49:50 | |
you find it very hard to control your emotions, you find it very hard not to get that need to nurture. | 0:49:50 | 0:49:55 | |
Wow, look at that! What a nice belly! | 0:50:01 | 0:50:03 | |
Oh, you're so cute, yes you are. Oh, yeah! | 0:50:05 | 0:50:11 | |
But responding to pets as though they were children can be seen in a very different light. | 0:50:11 | 0:50:16 | |
I think we can think of little puppies brought home as parasites. | 0:50:18 | 0:50:22 | |
They don't do anything useful, they're not perceived | 0:50:22 | 0:50:25 | |
as a food source, they're not perceived as a guard dog. | 0:50:25 | 0:50:27 | |
They are simply brought home for fun. | 0:50:27 | 0:50:29 | |
They are essentially moving our focus away from having children on to having pets. | 0:50:29 | 0:50:35 | |
I think it's safe to say that dogs have evolutionally been very successful. | 0:50:38 | 0:50:42 | |
If you compare them to wolves, you'll see that wolves are now an endangered species | 0:50:42 | 0:50:46 | |
while dogs, of course, are all around the world. | 0:50:46 | 0:50:49 | |
The cuckoo is perhaps quite a good analogy, because the baby cuckoo, of course, | 0:50:49 | 0:50:53 | |
being planted in somebody else's nest, prompts mother bird to look after baby cuckoo, | 0:50:53 | 0:50:59 | |
even though there's nothing in it for the mother bird at all. | 0:50:59 | 0:51:02 | |
They actually, through their behaviour, through their looks, get exactly what they want. | 0:51:02 | 0:51:10 | |
They may be parasitic in that we cannot help ourselves, but what we get | 0:51:10 | 0:51:13 | |
in return is probably sometimes much greater than what we put in. | 0:51:13 | 0:51:17 | |
Experiments have proved what dog owners have always suspected. | 0:51:33 | 0:51:37 | |
After thousands of years living together, dogs are attuned to us like no other animal. | 0:51:37 | 0:51:42 | |
New research has taken our understanding of how dogs evolved | 0:51:45 | 0:51:48 | |
to a whole new level and getting us closer to exactly what it means to be tame. | 0:51:48 | 0:51:54 | |
Now dogs could be about to provide us with the greatest gift of all. | 0:51:57 | 0:52:01 | |
When it comes to combating human disease, dogs could hold many of the answers. | 0:52:01 | 0:52:08 | |
They're going to help us tackle some of the most dangerous diseases | 0:52:08 | 0:52:13 | |
of our time that kill millions of people every year. | 0:52:13 | 0:52:15 | |
Dr Elinor Karlsson, a geneticist at the Brode Institute, Harvard, is on the hunt | 0:52:16 | 0:52:21 | |
for gene mutations that could throw light on human diseases. | 0:52:21 | 0:52:25 | |
I think there's hundreds of diseases that are in common between dogs and humans. | 0:52:27 | 0:52:32 | |
There's diabetes, there's various cardiac diseases, there's epilepsy, | 0:52:32 | 0:52:36 | |
there's a lot of different cancers - bone cancers, breast cancers, brain tumours. | 0:52:36 | 0:52:43 | |
The narrow gene pool within a dog breed | 0:52:43 | 0:52:45 | |
makes it far easier to pinpoint genetic mutations than in humans. | 0:52:45 | 0:52:49 | |
For more than 200 years, people have been making up all of these different breeds, | 0:52:49 | 0:52:54 | |
and now we can just use them to study genetics. | 0:52:54 | 0:52:57 | |
If you looked in a population of humans, all the people in a country like the UK, | 0:52:57 | 0:53:01 | |
you'd have quite a lot of genetic variation across them. People would be quite different from one another. | 0:53:01 | 0:53:06 | |
But within a breed, dogs are very similar to each other. | 0:53:06 | 0:53:09 | |
Particular dog breeds are prone to certain diseases, and this makes them incredibly useful to study. | 0:53:11 | 0:53:18 | |
Today, the team are taking blood samples from boxers, | 0:53:20 | 0:53:24 | |
a breed that is susceptible to a fatal heart disease called cardiomyopathy. | 0:53:24 | 0:53:27 | |
What happens is they have | 0:53:29 | 0:53:32 | |
irregular heartbeat, | 0:53:32 | 0:53:34 | |
and it compromises blood flow in their body, | 0:53:34 | 0:53:38 | |
so it can cause collapse and also it can cause sudden cardiac death. | 0:53:38 | 0:53:44 | |
It's an invisible disease that affects humans, too, causing sudden death in apparently healthy people. | 0:53:44 | 0:53:52 | |
The DNA in boxers' blood could hold vital clues to the genetic causes of the disease. | 0:53:52 | 0:53:59 | |
Dr Karlsson is part of the team that in 2005 mapped the dog genome, | 0:54:07 | 0:54:13 | |
all 2.4 billion letters of the dog's DNA code. | 0:54:13 | 0:54:17 | |
Once we had the dog genome sequence, we could design a gene chip, which would allow us to compare | 0:54:17 | 0:54:22 | |
all of our sick dogs and our healthy dogs and find the genes that are causing diseases. | 0:54:22 | 0:54:26 | |
Using a genotyping machine, Dr Karlsson is able to simultaneously analyse thousands of regions of DNA | 0:54:28 | 0:54:36 | |
from boxers with and without cardiomyopathy. | 0:54:36 | 0:54:41 | |
What you see when you compare sick dogs to healthy dogs and go across the genome from chromosome one | 0:54:41 | 0:54:46 | |
to chromosome two and across is that most of the points | 0:54:46 | 0:54:48 | |
are right near zero and there's not a lot of differences between the healthy dogs and the sick dogs, | 0:54:48 | 0:54:52 | |
until you get to chromosome 17, and there all of a sudden you have a huge number of differences. | 0:54:52 | 0:54:57 | |
This is exciting, because this means | 0:54:57 | 0:54:58 | |
this is the region of the genome that holds the gene causing our disease. | 0:54:58 | 0:55:02 | |
Karlsson's team have honed in on this region to pinpoint the exact gene. | 0:55:03 | 0:55:09 | |
We've found a gene related to sudden cardiac death. | 0:55:09 | 0:55:12 | |
We think there's another one because we haven't told the whole story yet. | 0:55:12 | 0:55:16 | |
But we think we know what the mutation is in that gene causing the disease. | 0:55:16 | 0:55:20 | |
Now the mutation has been identified, | 0:55:21 | 0:55:24 | |
the team have been able to locate the corresponding gene in humans. | 0:55:24 | 0:55:28 | |
It's accelerated a process that, without dogs, could have taken decades. | 0:55:28 | 0:55:33 | |
By knowing what gene is causing it in dogs, | 0:55:35 | 0:55:37 | |
we have an idea that this gene can cause this disease in humans. | 0:55:37 | 0:55:40 | |
I think that there's probably a lot of diseases that are so complicated in humans | 0:55:42 | 0:55:47 | |
that if we didn't have dogs it would take us a long time to start piecing it together. | 0:55:47 | 0:55:51 | |
Dogs basically give us a huge head start on that. | 0:55:51 | 0:55:54 | |
So I think this puts the benefits that dogs give us on a whole new level, and I think | 0:55:54 | 0:56:01 | |
if they can help us cure those diseases, then we can really say that dogs are good for our health. | 0:56:01 | 0:56:08 | |
It's a very important part of life to actually know a dog. | 0:56:13 | 0:56:17 | |
And especially a dog that adores you like this has got to be good for yourself. | 0:56:17 | 0:56:24 | |
It's kind of impossible to have a bad day | 0:56:24 | 0:56:26 | |
when you're coming home to a wet nose and a waggy tail, I think. | 0:56:26 | 0:56:29 | |
I can't imagine life without her. | 0:56:29 | 0:56:33 | |
It's quite strange. We weren't lacking anything before we had him, | 0:56:33 | 0:56:36 | |
and yet now we would feel that we were lacking if he wasn't here. | 0:56:36 | 0:56:39 | |
They just enrich your life. | 0:56:39 | 0:56:42 | |
They are the best thing ever. | 0:56:42 | 0:56:44 | |
They keep you young. | 0:56:44 | 0:56:45 | |
For a pet that's been around so long, | 0:56:47 | 0:56:49 | |
dog research is an astonishingly new area of science. | 0:56:49 | 0:56:53 | |
It's a very basic human need to have social relationships, | 0:56:53 | 0:56:58 | |
and one of the wonderful things about dogs, of course, is they offer you a way of giving unconditional love | 0:56:58 | 0:57:05 | |
and receiving unconditional love in the other end. | 0:57:05 | 0:57:08 | |
Dogs are the ones that live with us in the same environment. | 0:57:09 | 0:57:13 | |
They've been selected to live in this new environment, and they are specially tuned into humans, | 0:57:13 | 0:57:19 | |
so humans are their natural social partner. | 0:57:19 | 0:57:22 | |
But we're only just beginning to recognise their full potential. | 0:57:22 | 0:57:26 | |
Understanding dogs has the capacity to give us insights into disease, | 0:57:26 | 0:57:31 | |
the human mind and our very existence. | 0:57:31 | 0:57:34 | |
I think one reason that there are almost seven billion people on earth is in large part | 0:57:34 | 0:57:40 | |
due to the role that dogs have played in our evolutionary existence. | 0:57:40 | 0:57:43 | |
While we can have good relationships with a wide variety of animals, historically, our relationship | 0:57:45 | 0:57:51 | |
with dogs seems to have been the longest one with any domestic animal. | 0:57:51 | 0:57:54 | |
Personally, I don't think it's any coincidence that the dog is referred to as man's best friend. | 0:57:54 | 0:57:59 | |
Subtitles by Red Bee Media Ltd | 0:58:14 | 0:58:17 | |
E-mail [email protected] | 0:58:17 | 0:58:20 |