Browse content similar to Is Seeing Believing?. Check below for episodes and series from the same categories and more!
Line | From | To | |
---|---|---|---|
Welcome to the strange and wonderful world of illusions. | 0:00:06 | 0:00:10 | |
Baa. Baa. Baa. | 0:00:10 | 0:00:12 | |
Illusions to deceive your eyes. | 0:00:12 | 0:00:16 | |
So I do take cheques. | 0:00:16 | 0:00:18 | |
Trick your tongue. | 0:00:18 | 0:00:20 | |
And fool your sense of touch. | 0:00:22 | 0:00:26 | |
Oh! | 0:00:26 | 0:00:29 | |
But don't worry, it's all in the name of a noble scientific quest. | 0:00:29 | 0:00:36 | |
These illusions hold the key to how our senses work. | 0:00:36 | 0:00:40 | |
When you open your eyes in the morning, | 0:00:40 | 0:00:42 | |
most people think, "I'm seeing the world as it is". | 0:00:42 | 0:00:45 | |
The beautiful thing about illusions is they tell us that that's not true. | 0:00:45 | 0:00:49 | |
They show us that our perceptions of the world are something different | 0:00:49 | 0:00:53 | |
from seeing it as it really is. | 0:00:53 | 0:00:55 | |
Illusions are providing a unique window into the inner workings of our minds. | 0:00:58 | 0:01:03 | |
Helping to reveal what our sensory brains are really capable of. | 0:01:03 | 0:01:08 | |
So this is a golden age in perceptual psychology. | 0:01:08 | 0:01:11 | |
The things we've learned over the last ten years have been absolutely phenomenal. | 0:01:11 | 0:01:15 | |
These new discoveries are opening up a whole new world of possibility. | 0:01:15 | 0:01:20 | |
Even enabling us to move beyond our sensory evolution altogether. | 0:01:20 | 0:01:26 | |
So watch, play along, and prepare to be amazed at what your senses can do. | 0:01:31 | 0:01:37 | |
Can you trust anything you see with your eyes? | 0:01:51 | 0:01:55 | |
Do you think seeing is really believing? | 0:01:57 | 0:02:00 | |
The beautiful thing about illusions is that they make us realise | 0:02:02 | 0:02:06 | |
things are not always quite as they seem. | 0:02:06 | 0:02:09 | |
So the question for you is, you see the surface here and the surface there? | 0:02:12 | 0:02:17 | |
Right? Do they look the same in terms of their colour. | 0:02:17 | 0:02:19 | |
That's what you see, they look different. | 0:02:19 | 0:02:22 | |
What if I told you that they're actually the same. Will you put money on it? | 0:02:22 | 0:02:26 | |
Would you bet your life on it? | 0:02:26 | 0:02:28 | |
You wouldn't be standing here, would you?! | 0:02:28 | 0:02:31 | |
-Do they look the same now? -That's mad, ain't it? -It looks it. | 0:02:31 | 0:02:34 | |
Are they the same physically? | 0:02:34 | 0:02:35 | |
To our amazement and delight, illusions are so powerful | 0:02:35 | 0:02:39 | |
that even when we know how they work we can still be fooled. | 0:02:39 | 0:02:44 | |
But for scientists like Dr Beau Lotto, they are far more than fun and games. | 0:02:44 | 0:02:50 | |
Now, how many people see four blue tiles on the left? | 0:02:50 | 0:02:57 | |
Yes? I see four blue. How many people see seven yellow tiles on the right? | 0:02:57 | 0:03:02 | |
I see seven yellow. | 0:03:02 | 0:03:04 | |
What if I told you they're all grey? | 0:03:04 | 0:03:07 | |
So if I take, for instance, this tile, what colour is it now? | 0:03:07 | 0:03:12 | |
Grey. | 0:03:12 | 0:03:14 | |
If I move it over here, | 0:03:14 | 0:03:17 | |
what colour is it now? | 0:03:17 | 0:03:19 | |
Yellowish grey. | 0:03:19 | 0:03:21 | |
So those tiles are all physically the same. | 0:03:21 | 0:03:24 | |
Illusions are crucial tools that reveal how the world out there | 0:03:24 | 0:03:30 | |
can be very different to the one in our heads. | 0:03:30 | 0:03:33 | |
And it's this gap between reality and what we perceive that holds the key to how our senses work. | 0:03:33 | 0:03:42 | |
So if you're unsure if seeing is really believing, you're not the only one. | 0:03:45 | 0:03:52 | |
Is seeing believing? | 0:03:52 | 0:03:54 | |
What do you mean by that? | 0:03:54 | 0:03:56 | |
Is seeing believing? | 0:03:56 | 0:03:57 | |
Is seeing believing? | 0:03:59 | 0:04:01 | |
No. | 0:04:03 | 0:04:04 | |
Yes. | 0:04:04 | 0:04:06 | |
I think it depends who you ask. | 0:04:06 | 0:04:08 | |
Seeing is literally believing. | 0:04:08 | 0:04:10 | |
We see what we believe. | 0:04:10 | 0:04:12 | |
So, yes. | 0:04:14 | 0:04:15 | |
So it's this question of whether seeing really is believing that's | 0:04:19 | 0:04:22 | |
helping scientists to open up the fascinating world inside our heads. | 0:04:22 | 0:04:28 | |
And one of the places they are turning to for inspiration is an ancient and untapped source. | 0:04:34 | 0:04:41 | |
Magic Singh is a master of illusion. | 0:04:51 | 0:04:55 | |
His livelihood depends on his ability to confuse, trick and deceive. | 0:04:55 | 0:05:00 | |
It's something magicians like him have been doing for millennia. | 0:05:03 | 0:05:07 | |
But now scientists want in on the act. | 0:05:09 | 0:05:13 | |
Magicians have developed | 0:05:13 | 0:05:14 | |
really powerful ways of manipulating what we see. | 0:05:14 | 0:05:19 | |
And many of these techniques have been tried and tested in front of live audiences. | 0:05:19 | 0:05:24 | |
So by doing so, magicians have sort of developed | 0:05:24 | 0:05:27 | |
a very solid understanding of how we see the world. | 0:05:27 | 0:05:32 | |
Psychologist Gustav Kuhn is well versed in the language of illusion. | 0:05:32 | 0:05:38 | |
In a former life, he was a professional magician. | 0:05:38 | 0:05:41 | |
Today he's swapped the magic circuit for the science lab, | 0:05:43 | 0:05:46 | |
but he's convinced there are some important lessons | 0:05:46 | 0:05:49 | |
to be learnt from plundering the magician's book of tricks. | 0:05:49 | 0:05:53 | |
So actually take the card out. | 0:05:56 | 0:05:58 | |
We're really interested in the magic tricks per se, | 0:05:58 | 0:06:01 | |
but what we focus on is the techniques that magicians use to manipulate your perception. | 0:06:01 | 0:06:06 | |
OK, I'm not going to put the eye-tracker on you, so if you could just wear these glasses. | 0:06:06 | 0:06:10 | |
In order to find out how these illusions work, Gustav Kuhn has developed | 0:06:10 | 0:06:15 | |
an eye-tracking experiment | 0:06:15 | 0:06:16 | |
to enable him to find out what's happening when we watch certain tricks. | 0:06:16 | 0:06:21 | |
Now in the vanishing ball illusion, the magician tosses the ball up a couple of times and then on the | 0:06:29 | 0:06:35 | |
final throw, he just pretends to toss the ball up in the air. | 0:06:35 | 0:06:39 | |
Yet most people actually experience an imaginary ball | 0:06:39 | 0:06:42 | |
leave the hand and then sort of disappear somewhere up there. | 0:06:42 | 0:06:46 | |
But when Gustav analysed his data, | 0:06:46 | 0:06:49 | |
he discovered the eyes and the brain told a very different story. | 0:06:49 | 0:06:54 | |
Now, the eye-tracking data showed us that whilst most people were fooled by the illusion, | 0:06:56 | 0:07:00 | |
the eyes weren't tricked. | 0:07:00 | 0:07:02 | |
So the eyes, rather than actually looking at the imaginary ball, just stayed on the face. | 0:07:02 | 0:07:07 | |
And what this showed us is that the illusion really happened in people's minds. | 0:07:07 | 0:07:12 | |
What this trick really demonstrates is that, rather than seeing what's physically present, | 0:07:18 | 0:07:24 | |
the way we see the world is based on our prediction of the world. | 0:07:24 | 0:07:28 | |
So we see things that we expect to see, so in this case, | 0:07:28 | 0:07:31 | |
we expect the ball to leave the hand, and that's why | 0:07:31 | 0:07:34 | |
we actually see the ball, even though physically it's not actually present. | 0:07:34 | 0:07:39 | |
When it comes to what we see, the brain often overrules the eyes, | 0:07:43 | 0:07:48 | |
even constructing events that may not have actually happened. | 0:07:48 | 0:07:52 | |
It's an important insight into how our visual system operates in the real world. | 0:07:52 | 0:07:58 | |
In the real world things happen incredibly quickly and we have to | 0:07:58 | 0:08:02 | |
respond at great speed and accuracy to visual information. | 0:08:02 | 0:08:06 | |
This information processing may take up to 150 milliseconds, | 0:08:06 | 0:08:10 | |
and that kind of delay would just be far too great for us to miss, | 0:08:10 | 0:08:14 | |
for example, catching a ball or so. | 0:08:14 | 0:08:17 | |
So rather than just relying on this information, what the visual system does, | 0:08:17 | 0:08:22 | |
is it predicts what's going to be happening in the future. | 0:08:22 | 0:08:25 | |
So in many ways, what we see is what's going to happen in the future rather than in the present. | 0:08:25 | 0:08:30 | |
So seeing may not always be believing. | 0:08:34 | 0:08:38 | |
But is our sense of hearing any more reliable? | 0:08:38 | 0:08:42 | |
At any one moment, we are being bombarded by sensory information. | 0:08:58 | 0:09:03 | |
Our brains do a remarkable job of making sense of it all. | 0:09:04 | 0:09:08 | |
It seems easy enough to separate the sounds we hear | 0:09:11 | 0:09:17 | |
from the sights we see. | 0:09:17 | 0:09:18 | |
But there is one illusion that reveals this isn't always the case. | 0:09:22 | 0:09:27 | |
Baa, baa, baa. | 0:09:28 | 0:09:33 | |
Have a look at this. What do you hear? | 0:09:33 | 0:09:36 | |
Baa, baa, baa. | 0:09:36 | 0:09:39 | |
Baa, baa, baa. | 0:09:41 | 0:09:43 | |
But look what happens when we change the picture. | 0:09:43 | 0:09:47 | |
Faa, faa, faa. | 0:09:47 | 0:09:51 | |
Faa, faa, faa. | 0:09:51 | 0:09:55 | |
-Faa, faa... -And yet the sound hasn't changed. | 0:09:55 | 0:09:59 | |
In every clip, you are only ever hearing "Baa", with a B. | 0:09:59 | 0:10:04 | |
Baa, baa, baa. | 0:10:04 | 0:10:06 | |
Baa... | 0:10:06 | 0:10:08 | |
It's an illusion known as the McGurk effect. | 0:10:08 | 0:10:11 | |
Take another look. | 0:10:11 | 0:10:13 | |
-Baa, baa... -Concentrate first on the right of the screen. | 0:10:13 | 0:10:18 | |
Now to the left of the screen. | 0:10:18 | 0:10:22 | |
-Baa, baa... -The illusions occurs because what you are seeing clashes with what you are hearing. | 0:10:22 | 0:10:28 | |
In the illusion, what we see overrides what we hear, | 0:10:28 | 0:10:34 | |
so the mouth movements we see as we look at a face | 0:10:34 | 0:10:37 | |
can actually influence what we believe we're hearing. | 0:10:37 | 0:10:39 | |
If we close our eyes, we actually hear the sound as it is. | 0:10:39 | 0:10:42 | |
If we open our eyes, we actually see how the mouth movements can influence what we're hearing. | 0:10:42 | 0:10:47 | |
Baa, baa, baa. | 0:10:47 | 0:10:51 | |
It's a bizarre effect. | 0:10:51 | 0:10:53 | |
Remember, the only sound you're hearing is "Baa", with a B. | 0:10:53 | 0:10:57 | |
Faa, faa, faa. | 0:10:57 | 0:10:59 | |
Baa, baa, baa. | 0:10:59 | 0:11:02 | |
What's remarkable about this illusions is | 0:11:02 | 0:11:05 | |
even knowing how it's done doesn't seem to make a difference. | 0:11:05 | 0:11:09 | |
The effect works no matter how much you know about the effect. | 0:11:09 | 0:11:13 | |
I've been studying the McGurk effect for 25 years, | 0:11:13 | 0:11:16 | |
and I've been the face in the stimuli, | 0:11:16 | 0:11:18 | |
I've seen stimuli thousands and thousands of times, but the effect still works on me. | 0:11:18 | 0:11:22 | |
I can't help it, the speech brain takes in that information | 0:11:22 | 0:11:25 | |
and doesn't care about what outside knowledge you bring to bear. | 0:11:25 | 0:11:29 | |
Baa, baa, baa. | 0:11:29 | 0:11:33 | |
The McGurk effect shows us that what we hear may not always be the truth. | 0:11:33 | 0:11:39 | |
It also helps us to understand what happens when our senses conflict. | 0:11:39 | 0:11:45 | |
Baa, baa, baa. | 0:11:45 | 0:11:48 | |
When the brain has the conflicting information, | 0:11:48 | 0:11:50 | |
it tries to make sense of that conflict, | 0:11:50 | 0:11:52 | |
and depending on what type of modality is providing more, I guess, salient information, | 0:11:52 | 0:11:58 | |
that information might override or at least combine with the other information. | 0:11:58 | 0:12:02 | |
So we can't always trust what we hear because sometimes our sense of vision takes over, | 0:12:05 | 0:12:12 | |
enabling us to maintain a coherent view of the world. | 0:12:12 | 0:12:17 | |
But why do illusions have such power? | 0:12:22 | 0:12:25 | |
Scientist are finding answers in the most surprising of places. | 0:12:25 | 0:12:31 | |
For bees, colour is a matter of life and death. | 0:12:37 | 0:12:41 | |
They need to distinguish between colours to find the source of their food. | 0:12:41 | 0:12:45 | |
And the way bees learn this important lesson can offer us insights into how we perceive. | 0:12:45 | 0:12:53 | |
One of the great things about studying bees is that bees see colour much the way that we see it. | 0:12:54 | 0:13:00 | |
They see the same illusions that we see, yet they do it with only a million brain cells. | 0:13:00 | 0:13:07 | |
Which means that we can actually study how bumble bees see | 0:13:07 | 0:13:10 | |
and in doing so we can understand how we see. | 0:13:10 | 0:13:12 | |
Dr Beau Lotto has devised a unique experiment known as the bee matrix, | 0:13:15 | 0:13:20 | |
where he uses 64 coloured lights to represent flowers. | 0:13:20 | 0:13:25 | |
The aim of the game is to find the sugar reward. | 0:13:26 | 0:13:30 | |
We are training the bees to go to blue flowers, as opposed to purple flowers, | 0:13:31 | 0:13:35 | |
and the way we do that is we reward only the blue flowers, | 0:13:35 | 0:13:38 | |
reward being sugar water, and we don't put any reward into the purple flowers. | 0:13:38 | 0:13:42 | |
So if they land on a blue flower, they get a reward. | 0:13:42 | 0:13:45 | |
And then they associate that with the colour. | 0:13:45 | 0:13:48 | |
In the top part of the array we have blue flowers surrounded by white flowers, in the bottom part of | 0:13:48 | 0:13:54 | |
the array we have purple flowers also surrounded by white flowers. | 0:13:54 | 0:13:57 | |
Only the blue flowers have a sugar reward. | 0:13:57 | 0:14:00 | |
At first, the bees quickly find their reward by learning the difference between the colours. | 0:14:00 | 0:14:07 | |
But then things are made more difficult. | 0:14:09 | 0:14:11 | |
Using filters, Beau changes the colours, so now the blue and purple flowers look exactly the same. | 0:14:11 | 0:14:20 | |
But remarkably, the bees still fly straight to to the reward. | 0:14:20 | 0:14:26 | |
So as far as the bee's eye is concerned, those are exactly the same. | 0:14:29 | 0:14:34 | |
If the bees only remembered the colour of the stimulus, | 0:14:34 | 0:14:38 | |
they should go to both, because they're physically the same. | 0:14:38 | 0:14:41 | |
If, however, they've remembered the blue flowers in a context | 0:14:41 | 0:14:45 | |
and used that context, they should now go only go to the top. | 0:14:45 | 0:14:48 | |
What we've shown is that that's exactly what happens, which means they are using the context. | 0:14:48 | 0:14:53 | |
They have remembered and learned the relationships between the colours to solve the puzzle. | 0:14:53 | 0:14:58 | |
So, to solve the puzzle, the bees don't just look at the colours in the middle | 0:15:00 | 0:15:04 | |
they also look at the colours that surround them. | 0:15:04 | 0:15:09 | |
And it's by comparing the central colours to those on the outside | 0:15:09 | 0:15:13 | |
that they are able to detect the true colour. | 0:15:13 | 0:15:16 | |
It shows that, for bees, colour isn't just seen in isolation, | 0:15:16 | 0:15:21 | |
it's entirely dependent on the environment in which it's perceived. | 0:15:21 | 0:15:26 | |
And what applies to bees also applies to us, every day of our lives. | 0:15:26 | 0:15:33 | |
Here we have two cubes, except in this case | 0:15:38 | 0:15:41 | |
it looks as if the cube on the left is under yellow light, | 0:15:41 | 0:15:45 | |
and the cube on the right is under blue light. | 0:15:45 | 0:15:48 | |
On the left we have four blue tiles, and on the right we have seven yellow tiles. | 0:15:48 | 0:15:55 | |
What's amazing about this illusion | 0:15:57 | 0:15:59 | |
is that the blue tiles on the left are exactly the same, physically, | 0:15:59 | 0:16:04 | |
as the yellow tiles on the right. | 0:16:04 | 0:16:07 | |
They're all in fact grey. | 0:16:07 | 0:16:09 | |
So in this instance the brain has created colours that simply aren't there. | 0:16:09 | 0:16:14 | |
When the other colours are stripped away, we can see the blue and yellow tiles are just grey. | 0:16:15 | 0:16:22 | |
Put the scene back, and the colours change back to yellow and blue. | 0:16:22 | 0:16:28 | |
It shows that, in spite of our strongest instincts, colour is a purely subjective experience, | 0:16:37 | 0:16:45 | |
governed by the context in which we see it. | 0:16:45 | 0:16:48 | |
Redness is not a product of the world. | 0:16:50 | 0:16:53 | |
It doesn't exist unless we're there to make it. | 0:16:54 | 0:16:57 | |
Blueness is not a part of the world, wavelengths are not colour. | 0:16:57 | 0:17:01 | |
All they are is little packets of energy called photons, they are not colour. | 0:17:01 | 0:17:05 | |
We take that and we make perceptions of them, and those perceptions guide our behaviour. | 0:17:07 | 0:17:13 | |
Illusions fool us because, try as we might, | 0:17:13 | 0:17:16 | |
we cannot overcome our experience of how we think the world works. | 0:17:16 | 0:17:23 | |
It's these experiences we store in our heads that really determines what we see. | 0:17:23 | 0:17:30 | |
What's amazing is that that information, coming from the eyes | 0:17:30 | 0:17:35 | |
through the thalamus to the back part of the brain, | 0:17:35 | 0:17:38 | |
actually only makes up 10% of the overall information we use to see. | 0:17:38 | 0:17:44 | |
The rest of the information comes from other parts of the brain. | 0:17:44 | 0:17:49 | |
Only 10% of the information we use to see comes from the eyes. | 0:17:49 | 0:17:53 | |
But are these experiences only built up in the course of our own lives, | 0:17:53 | 0:18:00 | |
or are some illusions so powerful | 0:18:00 | 0:18:03 | |
their roots lie far in our distant past? | 0:18:03 | 0:18:08 | |
Janine Spencer and husband Justin O'Brien are hoping to find out | 0:18:26 | 0:18:31 | |
if seeing certain illusions is learnt in the course of our lives | 0:18:31 | 0:18:35 | |
or hard-wired from birth. | 0:18:35 | 0:18:37 | |
It requires several babies, and a great deal of patience. | 0:18:37 | 0:18:43 | |
Anybody who studies with babies will know they're notoriously difficult, | 0:18:43 | 0:18:47 | |
not because they're hard in themselves, they're lovely, | 0:18:47 | 0:18:50 | |
I love having babies in the lab, but we can't ask them anything. | 0:18:50 | 0:18:53 | |
There is a number of reasons why it takes such a long time to get baby data, | 0:18:56 | 0:19:01 | |
pretty normal reasons. | 0:19:01 | 0:19:03 | |
They cry, they get hungry, they don't like what they're looking at, | 0:19:03 | 0:19:06 | |
they want to move, they don't want to sit in their car seat, | 0:19:06 | 0:19:09 | |
so there are a number of reasons and because of that, | 0:19:09 | 0:19:12 | |
we have to test lots of babies to get enough data. | 0:19:12 | 0:19:15 | |
BABIES CRY | 0:19:15 | 0:19:17 | |
In order to find out they are using a famous piece of visual trickery | 0:19:19 | 0:19:25 | |
called the hollow mask illusion. | 0:19:25 | 0:19:28 | |
One side of the mask is hollow, but it doesn't necessarily look that way. | 0:19:28 | 0:19:33 | |
Even when we know it's an illusion, we see it as the convex face. | 0:19:35 | 0:19:40 | |
Even when we know it's hollow our visual system sees it, we interpret it, as being convex. | 0:19:40 | 0:19:47 | |
Essentially, our knowledge of faces is overriding what our visual system can see, | 0:19:47 | 0:19:54 | |
so our depth perception can see that it's hollow, | 0:19:54 | 0:19:56 | |
but our visual system is overriding that and saying, "no". | 0:19:56 | 0:19:59 | |
Not in those words, | 0:19:59 | 0:20:01 | |
it's unconscious of course, but that's a face, so it must be sticking out. | 0:20:01 | 0:20:06 | |
They've been testing the hollow mask illusion on babies at just four and a half months of age. | 0:20:06 | 0:20:13 | |
I think it's important to study babies to look at visual illusions | 0:20:13 | 0:20:17 | |
and any phenomenon where you want to find out if it's innate or not, | 0:20:17 | 0:20:21 | |
because they don't have that kind of experience we would get as adults. | 0:20:21 | 0:20:25 | |
The younger you can test them, the better it is. | 0:20:25 | 0:20:28 | |
It's known babies are good at recognising faces, but the question is, | 0:20:31 | 0:20:35 | |
do they still see a face when they look at the hollow side of the mask? | 0:20:35 | 0:20:41 | |
By carefully monitoring their eye movements, it's possible to detect if the babies see the illusion | 0:20:41 | 0:20:47 | |
by the way the mask captures their attention. | 0:20:47 | 0:20:50 | |
We measure the amount of interest the baby has in the experiment by looking at their eyes. | 0:20:52 | 0:20:57 | |
We monitor their eye movements and we time how long they look, | 0:20:57 | 0:21:01 | |
and when we get to a certain level where they're not looking very much at all, | 0:21:01 | 0:21:05 | |
we know they're bored of the experiment. | 0:21:05 | 0:21:07 | |
It's still early days, but 50 babies later a pattern is beginning to emerge. | 0:21:11 | 0:21:18 | |
From the data we have so far, it would suggest that babies can see the illusion, | 0:21:19 | 0:21:25 | |
giving an indication that face perception is an innate ability. | 0:21:25 | 0:21:30 | |
If this pattern continues, it will be the first significant evidence to suggest that seeing | 0:21:30 | 0:21:36 | |
certain illusions is so powerful it's an ability we've inherited from our parents. | 0:21:36 | 0:21:43 | |
What's exciting for us about the results we're finding at the moment | 0:21:43 | 0:21:47 | |
is that it doesn't just tell us about the way babies see, it tells us about our evolutionary past. | 0:21:47 | 0:21:53 | |
The experience of our ancestors of seeing faces and them being so necessary for survival | 0:21:53 | 0:21:59 | |
is now written into our DNA, so when a baby's born, | 0:21:59 | 0:22:02 | |
the first thing they'll look at and show interest in is a face. | 0:22:02 | 0:22:04 | |
The hollow mask illusion helps explain why seeing illusions may have come about in the first place. | 0:22:07 | 0:22:13 | |
We've learnt to see what best aids our chances of survival. | 0:22:15 | 0:22:20 | |
In our evolutionary past, it's important to see faces | 0:22:22 | 0:22:25 | |
because they could be our enemy. | 0:22:25 | 0:22:27 | |
It's also not just human faces, it's animal faces as well, | 0:22:27 | 0:22:31 | |
so we're very good at seeing animal faces. | 0:22:31 | 0:22:34 | |
So something staring at you through the trees could be a tiger, | 0:22:34 | 0:22:38 | |
so it's important you interpret something as being a face. | 0:22:38 | 0:22:41 | |
If it turns out to be a pattern of leaves in the sunlight, you haven't lost anything. | 0:22:41 | 0:22:46 | |
If you ignore it and it really was a face, then you're in trouble. | 0:22:46 | 0:22:50 | |
For Janine and Justin, | 0:22:50 | 0:22:52 | |
over five years of infinite patience is finally paying off. | 0:22:52 | 0:22:58 | |
Yes, very pleased, we need more babies but we're very pleased with the results we have so far. | 0:22:58 | 0:23:03 | |
It's only taken five and a half years but we're nearly there. | 0:23:03 | 0:23:07 | |
Illusions show us that we literally see the world through the lens of the past, | 0:23:12 | 0:23:16 | |
learning to see in the way that's most useful to us. | 0:23:16 | 0:23:21 | |
It's an ability that's so important | 0:23:23 | 0:23:25 | |
it's been handed down through the generations for thousands of years. | 0:23:25 | 0:23:29 | |
So if you thought being tricked by illusions was a weakness, then think again. | 0:23:42 | 0:23:48 | |
They may seem to be just a bit of fun, but it turns out they may be the key to our success. | 0:23:48 | 0:23:56 | |
So what if I told you they're exactly the same? | 0:23:56 | 0:24:00 | |
-Oh, my God. -So, I do take cheques. | 0:24:00 | 0:24:03 | |
You just lost so much money! | 0:24:03 | 0:24:06 | |
Many people think that illusions in fact demonstrate the fragility | 0:24:06 | 0:24:10 | |
of the human senses, which is in fact completely rubbish. | 0:24:10 | 0:24:13 | |
Illusions don't tell us that our senses are fragile. | 0:24:13 | 0:24:18 | |
If they were, we wouldn't be here. Illusions tell us that actually, | 0:24:18 | 0:24:22 | |
our brains are incredibly capable of constructing meaning | 0:24:22 | 0:24:27 | |
from the meaningless. We're really good at doing that. | 0:24:27 | 0:24:31 | |
If we were to process all of the information | 0:24:31 | 0:24:34 | |
that we feel that we're aware of, | 0:24:34 | 0:24:36 | |
we would have to grow huge brains | 0:24:36 | 0:24:39 | |
and have massive heads that our bodies would just fail to support. | 0:24:39 | 0:24:44 | |
Rather than using this approach, we've evolved to, I think, | 0:24:44 | 0:24:49 | |
a very clever attentional system that only processes the information that is actually needed. | 0:24:49 | 0:24:55 | |
So, far from being a disadvantage, | 0:24:58 | 0:25:01 | |
illusions are a necessary and powerful shortcut | 0:25:01 | 0:25:05 | |
that lie at the heart of our most sophisticated human abilities. | 0:25:05 | 0:25:10 | |
And yet the insights we can get from illusions don't end there. | 0:25:12 | 0:25:18 | |
Scientists are now realising that illusions get even more fascinating | 0:25:18 | 0:25:22 | |
when the senses start to work together. | 0:25:22 | 0:25:25 | |
One of the things most of us can hold on to is that our five senses work separately. | 0:25:32 | 0:25:38 | |
We see with our eyes, hear with our ears, | 0:25:38 | 0:25:42 | |
taste with our tongue and touch with our skin. | 0:25:42 | 0:25:46 | |
But scientists have been studying a group of people | 0:25:46 | 0:25:51 | |
for whom this just isn't the case. | 0:25:51 | 0:25:54 | |
So when I hear the sea, | 0:25:54 | 0:25:56 | |
the big clunkiness, as it were of the wave, | 0:25:56 | 0:26:00 | |
has a kind of dark, dark blue. | 0:26:00 | 0:26:05 | |
And the pebbley bit has kind of oranges and yellows | 0:26:05 | 0:26:10 | |
and little bits of white. | 0:26:10 | 0:26:13 | |
I heard the wind earlier and it had these kind of long shapes, | 0:26:16 | 0:26:22 | |
a bit like, you know, mackerel fillets. | 0:26:22 | 0:26:25 | |
You know those shapes, but a bit... The little thin ones. | 0:26:25 | 0:26:29 | |
Like that, but blue, and quite a lot of them going across. | 0:26:29 | 0:26:35 | |
I have synaesthesia, | 0:26:38 | 0:26:40 | |
which means when I experience taste, smell, sound, | 0:26:40 | 0:26:46 | |
I get visual images. | 0:26:46 | 0:26:48 | |
Shape, colour and texture to accompany the sense. | 0:26:48 | 0:26:52 | |
Synaesthesia is a mixing of the senses. | 0:26:54 | 0:26:58 | |
A sensory experience in one sense can trigger an entirely different reaction in another. | 0:26:58 | 0:27:03 | |
Whenever Philippa hears, smells, or tastes something, she also sees colours and shapes. | 0:27:05 | 0:27:12 | |
If I was to have fish and chips, the fish - | 0:27:12 | 0:27:16 | |
the crispiness, that's angular and then the actually taste | 0:27:16 | 0:27:22 | |
of the fish is kind of speckeldy brown. | 0:27:22 | 0:27:26 | |
Nice brown, but yeah, kind of coffee-coloured brown. | 0:27:26 | 0:27:31 | |
While this experience isn't always pleasurable, | 0:27:34 | 0:27:38 | |
it's helped drive Philippa's artistic creativity. | 0:27:38 | 0:27:42 | |
This is a painting of the taste of English mustard. | 0:27:43 | 0:27:48 | |
It has such a distinctive colour as it is as a product, | 0:27:49 | 0:27:54 | |
it's bright yellow, but when you taste it, to me, | 0:27:54 | 0:27:57 | |
it has this massive red hit, which then just disappears into something else | 0:27:57 | 0:28:06 | |
that ends up, by the time the kind of fumes of it | 0:28:06 | 0:28:10 | |
are going up through your nose, it's actually quite...pretty. | 0:28:10 | 0:28:15 | |
So it starts off as a big, massive red hit of taste, which then disperses into something else. | 0:28:15 | 0:28:22 | |
For years, synaesthesia wasn't taken seriously... | 0:28:26 | 0:28:29 | |
but now scientists are realising that people like Philippa | 0:28:32 | 0:28:36 | |
provide important clues as to the way all our senses work. | 0:28:36 | 0:28:43 | |
We're going to present you with letters and numbers. | 0:28:43 | 0:28:46 | |
They're, um, going to be coloured either red or green. | 0:28:46 | 0:28:49 | |
Dr Noam Sagiv has spent his career studying synaesthetes | 0:28:49 | 0:28:55 | |
in an attempt to understand how their brains are connected. | 0:28:55 | 0:28:59 | |
So scientists have suspected | 0:29:01 | 0:29:02 | |
for a long time that what causes synaesthesia is extra connections | 0:29:02 | 0:29:07 | |
between different parts of the brain, particularly between the sensory areas that are involved. | 0:29:07 | 0:29:12 | |
For example, if someone has auditory-visual synaesthesia, | 0:29:12 | 0:29:16 | |
we expect that the auditory part of the brain and the visual part of the brain would be cross-wired. | 0:29:16 | 0:29:23 | |
And this understanding of how their brains are wired has led to | 0:29:23 | 0:29:26 | |
an exciting new idea about the way all our senses develop from birth. | 0:29:26 | 0:29:32 | |
OK? | 0:29:32 | 0:29:34 | |
-That's it. -I got one wrong. | 0:29:34 | 0:29:36 | |
What we do know is that the brains of newborns are actually a lot more connected than the brains of adults. | 0:29:36 | 0:29:46 | |
We start our lives with a lot of connections in our brains and we lose some of them. | 0:29:46 | 0:29:51 | |
One of the ideas that is trying explain the difference | 0:29:51 | 0:29:55 | |
between synaesthetes' and non-synaesthetes' development | 0:29:55 | 0:30:00 | |
is that essentially, synaesthetes were able to keep | 0:30:00 | 0:30:04 | |
a little bit more of those many connections that we all started our lives with. | 0:30:04 | 0:30:09 | |
So this condition might have been something we've all had at one time in our lives, but since lost. | 0:30:09 | 0:30:17 | |
But the similarities between synaesthetes and the rest of us may not end there. | 0:30:17 | 0:30:24 | |
Scientists are now beginning to suspect that even as adults, | 0:30:24 | 0:30:29 | |
we may have far more in common with synaesthetes than we realise. | 0:30:29 | 0:30:33 | |
I can't imagine what it would be like to be alive | 0:30:33 | 0:30:36 | |
without it... | 0:30:36 | 0:30:39 | |
because it doesn't impose itself, it's just part of my being. | 0:30:39 | 0:30:43 | |
So I... | 0:30:43 | 0:30:43 | |
kind of don't believe that | 0:30:43 | 0:30:46 | |
people don't have it. | 0:30:46 | 0:30:48 | |
I think they're just not looking hard enough. | 0:30:48 | 0:30:51 | |
And this question of the way our senses are connected | 0:30:53 | 0:30:56 | |
is being answered with the help of another set of bizarre illusions. | 0:30:56 | 0:31:01 | |
Neuroscientist Charles Spence has recruited some willing volunteers for an unusual multi-sensory feast. | 0:31:18 | 0:31:25 | |
He's taken his science out of the lab and is going to attempt to trick | 0:31:25 | 0:31:31 | |
a group of trainee chefs, who rely on their senses more than most of us. | 0:31:31 | 0:31:36 | |
OK. You've got four coloured drinks in front of you and what I want you to do is to taste each one | 0:31:36 | 0:31:45 | |
and try and figure out what the flavour is. | 0:31:45 | 0:31:48 | |
The colours and flavours of the drinks have been mismatched, | 0:31:53 | 0:31:57 | |
resulting in a certain degree of confusion. | 0:31:57 | 0:32:00 | |
Just looking at some of the expressions on their faces, you can see confusion and puzzlement. | 0:32:03 | 0:32:09 | |
One of the people thinks that the yellow drink is apple. | 0:32:12 | 0:32:16 | |
It was actually strawberry. | 0:32:16 | 0:32:18 | |
And the red one, they smell like berries, but in fact was lemon. | 0:32:22 | 0:32:26 | |
I think the green one tasted more of lime. | 0:32:30 | 0:32:34 | |
Like lime cordial or something like that, rather than mint. | 0:32:34 | 0:32:37 | |
OK. Excellent. | 0:32:37 | 0:32:39 | |
Green lime, so it's completely lost the peppermint flavour | 0:32:39 | 0:32:43 | |
-and it's being completely driven by the eyes. -The light green actually | 0:32:43 | 0:32:47 | |
reminded me of green washing-up liquid rather than mint. | 0:32:47 | 0:32:51 | |
Lady's convinced it's washing-up liquid smell. | 0:32:51 | 0:32:53 | |
And that expectation and knowledge that comes | 0:32:53 | 0:32:55 | |
from names, from labels, from colours, from textures, | 0:32:55 | 0:32:58 | |
from ways of presentation, our brains use that all the time | 0:32:58 | 0:33:02 | |
to tell us what the flavour is. | 0:33:02 | 0:33:03 | |
People will talk about you eat with your eyes, which is probably much more true than we realise. | 0:33:03 | 0:33:09 | |
So it's impossible to separate what we see from what we taste. | 0:33:13 | 0:33:19 | |
But what may be even more surprising is that when it comes to what you eat, | 0:33:19 | 0:33:23 | |
your ears may be just as important. | 0:33:23 | 0:33:27 | |
CRUNCHING | 0:33:27 | 0:33:29 | |
This time the chefs are eating crisps, | 0:33:33 | 0:33:36 | |
but they are also hearing the sound of their own crunch via headphones. | 0:33:38 | 0:33:44 | |
But what they don't realise is the noises they hear have been changed. | 0:33:44 | 0:33:49 | |
When they hear low frequencies, they are tricked into thinking the crisps | 0:33:49 | 0:33:54 | |
are significantly less crunchy than when they hear higher frequencies. | 0:33:54 | 0:33:58 | |
When anyone thinks about flavour, the first sense they think about is taste. | 0:33:59 | 0:34:03 | |
To think about it a bit more, some people say, "Well I suppose smell's involved, too." | 0:34:03 | 0:34:08 | |
Then they start, possibly if pushed they'll say, "Well maybe colour's got something to do with it." | 0:34:08 | 0:34:14 | |
And finally a bit of texture. | 0:34:14 | 0:34:15 | |
Is it soft and slimy or crispy or crunchy? | 0:34:15 | 0:34:18 | |
But virtually no-one ever thinks about sound. | 0:34:18 | 0:34:20 | |
The results show hearing can have a significant effect on taste. | 0:34:20 | 0:34:26 | |
Just playing higher frequencies makes people believe crisps to be over 15% crispier. | 0:34:27 | 0:34:34 | |
But all this culinary trickery has even more insights to offer. | 0:34:36 | 0:34:43 | |
The reason these tricks work is because it's impossible to separate one sense from another. | 0:34:43 | 0:34:49 | |
It's experiments like these that have enabled scientists | 0:34:50 | 0:34:54 | |
to piece together a revolutionary new understanding of the brain. | 0:34:54 | 0:34:58 | |
The traditional view was that you had five senses on the outside, | 0:34:58 | 0:35:03 | |
and the eyes are connected to one bit of the brain, your ears are connected to a different part, | 0:35:03 | 0:35:07 | |
your skin to somewhere else. | 0:35:07 | 0:35:09 | |
Each sense had its own bit of brain. | 0:35:09 | 0:35:11 | |
What we find now is in fact, the eye is talking to the ear almost as soon | 0:35:11 | 0:35:14 | |
as those signals get from the eye and the ear into the brain. | 0:35:14 | 0:35:18 | |
From very early on, there are multisensory interactions at work. | 0:35:18 | 0:35:20 | |
Scientists are saying there is no such thing as a visual brain, | 0:35:20 | 0:35:23 | |
no such part of the brain that is just doing hearing. | 0:35:23 | 0:35:26 | |
All of the brain is multisensory, all of the brain is combining all the different senses, all the time. | 0:35:26 | 0:35:31 | |
So it turns out we are all far more similar to synaesthetes than we've realised. | 0:35:33 | 0:35:38 | |
It's clear we should no longer think of our senses | 0:35:38 | 0:35:42 | |
as working independently but as working together as one. | 0:35:42 | 0:35:48 | |
It's a discovery that has truly revolutionary possibilities. | 0:35:48 | 0:35:54 | |
-Hi. I'm Larry. -Hi, Edie. | 0:36:03 | 0:36:04 | |
Very nice to meet you. We're going to do little demonstration here called the rubber hand illusion. | 0:36:04 | 0:36:09 | |
This illusion may look like fairground fun, but it reveals | 0:36:09 | 0:36:12 | |
one of the most important new ideas in brain science. | 0:36:12 | 0:36:18 | |
Right there. | 0:36:18 | 0:36:20 | |
Good. Can you put this hand down right over here, and curl it up like the rubber hand is curled up | 0:36:20 | 0:36:26 | |
a little bit. I'm going to try to position the rubber hand so it looks like it's your own. | 0:36:26 | 0:36:31 | |
-Could you imagine that being your own hand? -Yeah. | 0:36:31 | 0:36:34 | |
We're going to stroke your finger simultaneously, the rubber finger and your real finger. | 0:36:34 | 0:36:40 | |
Hopefully this will convince you that the rubber hand is your own. Your brain will adopt this hand. | 0:36:40 | 0:36:46 | |
In the illusion, simply watching the rubber hand being stroked at the | 0:36:46 | 0:36:51 | |
-same time as the real hand is enough to trick the brain into adopting it as its own. -We like weird! | 0:36:51 | 0:36:58 | |
And slowly but surely, you should feel that the hand you're looking at is actually part of your body. | 0:37:02 | 0:37:08 | |
It feels like you're touching my hand with that one. | 0:37:08 | 0:37:10 | |
Right, so it feels like this is your hand I'm touching, right? | 0:37:10 | 0:37:14 | |
-Are you OK? -Yeah. -Good. | 0:37:19 | 0:37:21 | |
Try that at home with your kids! | 0:37:21 | 0:37:23 | |
The rubber hand illusion is a wonderful example of how | 0:37:23 | 0:37:26 | |
multisensory perception can influence how we perceive our own body. | 0:37:26 | 0:37:31 | |
That's how deep multisensory perception runs. | 0:37:31 | 0:37:34 | |
When you hold your hand out, it's generally thought that you know it's there because of the information | 0:37:34 | 0:37:40 | |
you're getting from your muscles and tendons and that sort of thing. | 0:37:40 | 0:37:43 | |
The rubber hand illusion shows how that can be overridden by visual information. | 0:37:43 | 0:37:49 | |
The rubber hand illusion shows the powerful connection between what we see and what we feel. | 0:37:49 | 0:37:56 | |
But it reveals even more than simply the way our senses are connected. | 0:37:56 | 0:38:00 | |
It hints that a fundamental change in the brain is taking place. | 0:38:02 | 0:38:07 | |
Oh! | 0:38:07 | 0:38:10 | |
-Isn't that strange?! -Yeah, that's creepy. | 0:38:11 | 0:38:14 | |
What might be going on in this illusion is that | 0:38:14 | 0:38:16 | |
the brain is actually changing to accommodate the new rubber hand. | 0:38:16 | 0:38:20 | |
Going through some sort of structural change that we call neuroplasticity. | 0:38:20 | 0:38:26 | |
Neuroplasticity is an exciting new idea that suggests the brain can change in response to experience. | 0:38:27 | 0:38:37 | |
And this is what's taking place in the rubber hand illusion. | 0:38:37 | 0:38:41 | |
The brain may be temporarily re-wiring itself to adopt the plastic hand as its own. | 0:38:41 | 0:38:48 | |
-It really feeling like it's your hand now, huh? -Yes. | 0:38:48 | 0:38:50 | |
Is that a little weird? | 0:38:50 | 0:38:52 | |
-Yes. -We like weird in perceptual psychology! | 0:38:52 | 0:38:55 | |
Here we go. | 0:38:55 | 0:38:57 | |
-Was that scary? -Yes. | 0:38:59 | 0:39:01 | |
Good, we like that! | 0:39:01 | 0:39:02 | |
'Brain plasticity is a terrifically exciting' | 0:39:02 | 0:39:04 | |
sort of phenomenon for perceptual psychology. | 0:39:04 | 0:39:07 | |
I think the rubber hand illusion shows that. | 0:39:07 | 0:39:10 | |
That the brain can change, based on a new experience. | 0:39:10 | 0:39:13 | |
This is important for somebody, say, who doesn't have vision, to know that they can compensate | 0:39:13 | 0:39:19 | |
through plasticity with another sense and use that to navigate the world. | 0:39:19 | 0:39:23 | |
This idea of a plastic, flexible brain is so exciting | 0:39:27 | 0:39:32 | |
because of the phenomenal possibilities it contains. | 0:39:32 | 0:39:35 | |
Not only do our senses work together, but it suggests one could be used to replace another. | 0:39:35 | 0:39:42 | |
CLICKING | 0:39:44 | 0:39:47 | |
I lost my first eye at the age of seven months, and my second | 0:40:00 | 0:40:04 | |
at the age of 13 months, to retinoblastoma, which is a retinal tumour. | 0:40:04 | 0:40:11 | |
I have no visual memories at all. | 0:40:13 | 0:40:15 | |
CLICKING | 0:40:15 | 0:40:16 | |
Although Daniel is completely blind, he's developed a remarkable ability | 0:40:16 | 0:40:21 | |
to see, using his sense of hearing alone. | 0:40:21 | 0:40:25 | |
CLICKS HIS TONGUE | 0:40:25 | 0:40:27 | |
People do express surprise | 0:40:30 | 0:40:33 | |
at a blind person cycling. | 0:40:33 | 0:40:37 | |
I think different people are good at different things. | 0:40:37 | 0:40:40 | |
I was good at cycling but I wasn't much for ball sports. | 0:40:42 | 0:40:45 | |
Using the sound of his tongue clicks, Daniel has learned to echolocate, just like a bat. | 0:40:47 | 0:40:54 | |
Echolocation is just another way of seeing. | 0:40:57 | 0:41:00 | |
It's a way of seeing with sound instead of light. | 0:41:00 | 0:41:03 | |
You extract images from the patterns of sound as they reflect off the environment. | 0:41:05 | 0:41:13 | |
When Daniel clicks, the sound waves he produces bounce off nearby objects. | 0:41:13 | 0:41:19 | |
From the returning echoes, Daniel creates an image in his mind, which he uses to navigate the world. | 0:41:19 | 0:41:28 | |
It's an ability that's enabled him to overcome the impossible. | 0:41:28 | 0:41:32 | |
I could cycle without echolocating for a brief while, and then it would end uncomfortably. | 0:41:32 | 0:41:39 | |
It's kind of like they say, | 0:41:41 | 0:41:44 | |
"Falling is really quite a blast, it's the striking the ground that's the real bummer." So, yeah... | 0:41:44 | 0:41:51 | |
But Daniel can show us far more than what one extraordinary man can achieve. | 0:41:56 | 0:42:02 | |
His remarkable bat-like abilities are helping scientists reveal the hidden potential of the brain. | 0:42:05 | 0:42:11 | |
Professor Lutz Wiegrebe is an expert in bat echolocation, but now in Daniel, | 0:42:13 | 0:42:19 | |
he's been given the unique opportunity to study his first human subject. | 0:42:19 | 0:42:23 | |
Wow. That is very cool. | 0:42:23 | 0:42:27 | |
There's another one! | 0:42:29 | 0:42:31 | |
Today, Lutz is conducting a series of MRI scans, | 0:42:35 | 0:42:39 | |
to find out what happens inside Daniel's brain when he echolocates. | 0:42:39 | 0:42:45 | |
For me personally, this has been a really great experience, because I've been working on the | 0:42:45 | 0:42:50 | |
echolocation of bats and we've only recently started working | 0:42:50 | 0:42:55 | |
on echolocation with humans. | 0:42:55 | 0:42:57 | |
Having Daniel around is like almost being able to talk to a bat, and Daniel is not only exceptionally | 0:42:57 | 0:43:04 | |
good at echolocation, he's also exceptionally good at verbalising how he does it. | 0:43:04 | 0:43:10 | |
Inside the scanner, Daniel is hearing virtual echoes. | 0:43:10 | 0:43:14 | |
This should enable the team to see which parts of his brain | 0:43:19 | 0:43:22 | |
are activated when he echolocates in the real world. | 0:43:22 | 0:43:27 | |
Lutz suspects that when Daniel clicks, something remarkable may be happening inside his brain. | 0:43:28 | 0:43:36 | |
Even though he can't see, the sounds he hears may still be activating parts of his visual brain. | 0:43:36 | 0:43:45 | |
What we are interested in is so-called cross-modal plasticity, which means that these parts | 0:43:45 | 0:43:51 | |
of the principal visual cortex are taken over by auditory information. | 0:43:51 | 0:43:57 | |
Lutz is looking for evidence to show just how malleable the human brain really is. | 0:43:59 | 0:44:06 | |
Not only can experience temporarily change the brain, | 0:44:08 | 0:44:12 | |
but as Daniel seems to suggest, these changes can also be permanent. | 0:44:12 | 0:44:19 | |
It means that at the extreme | 0:44:19 | 0:44:23 | |
extent of... | 0:44:23 | 0:44:25 | |
the cross-modal plasticity on a perceptual level that Daniel | 0:44:27 | 0:44:31 | |
has demonstrated, he can really see with his ears. | 0:44:31 | 0:44:35 | |
That it's not only that he can process spatial information acquired with his auditory system, but that | 0:44:35 | 0:44:41 | |
he can also recruit parts of his visual cortex to do this task. | 0:44:41 | 0:44:47 | |
It's just a demonstration how | 0:44:47 | 0:44:50 | |
plastic the system is, and how intelligently it's designed. | 0:44:50 | 0:44:55 | |
If one part of the brain has really no input any more because | 0:44:55 | 0:44:59 | |
of a sensory deprivation, then this part can be taken over by other modalities. | 0:44:59 | 0:45:06 | |
As unique individuals like Daniel seem to show, | 0:45:07 | 0:45:10 | |
the human brain can change and adapt in the most phenomenal way. | 0:45:10 | 0:45:15 | |
This has implications not only for people whose senses are impaired, | 0:45:17 | 0:45:21 | |
it has the potential to affect us all. | 0:45:23 | 0:45:26 | |
For Dr Angus Rupert, finding a way of replacing one sense with another has been a lifelong ambition. | 0:45:41 | 0:45:49 | |
It's something that for his colleagues at Fort Rucker Aviation Centre in Alabama | 0:45:49 | 0:45:54 | |
could mean the difference between life and death. | 0:45:54 | 0:45:57 | |
Since 1990 alone, we've lost between ten and 30 pilots and air crew per year, | 0:45:57 | 0:46:03 | |
just due to spatial disorientation. | 0:46:03 | 0:46:06 | |
These are the figures across our army, navy and air force. | 0:46:06 | 0:46:10 | |
We define spatial disorientation as occurs whenever a pilot misperceives | 0:46:10 | 0:46:14 | |
the position, motion or attitude of his aircraft, relative to the Earth or other significant objects. | 0:46:14 | 0:46:23 | |
In other words it's, "Which way is up?" | 0:46:23 | 0:46:25 | |
In normal circumstances, pilots can correct the problem of spatial disorientation by using their eyes. | 0:46:25 | 0:46:32 | |
But there are instances when they can't always rely on what they see. | 0:46:32 | 0:46:38 | |
When you are flying, there is no way for you to know where down is | 0:46:38 | 0:46:43 | |
unless you are actually looking at the horizon | 0:46:43 | 0:46:46 | |
or looking at an indicator | 0:46:46 | 0:46:48 | |
to give you the information in the aircraft. | 0:46:48 | 0:46:51 | |
But Angus Rupert thinks he has found the solution. | 0:46:54 | 0:46:59 | |
It comes in the form of the Tactile Situation Awareness System, or TSAS, | 0:46:59 | 0:47:04 | |
which uses touch to support or replace the sense of vision. | 0:47:04 | 0:47:10 | |
Together with research pilot John Ramiccio, | 0:47:10 | 0:47:13 | |
they've found a way of giving pilots spatial awareness by using a series of vibrating pads called tactors. | 0:47:13 | 0:47:21 | |
So, in this situation we have John wearing tactors incorporated into the shoulder harness. | 0:47:26 | 0:47:32 | |
These are the ones telling him if he is too high. | 0:47:32 | 0:47:34 | |
In the seat we have tactors letting him know if he's getting too low | 0:47:34 | 0:47:39 | |
as well as tactors around his waist here. | 0:47:39 | 0:47:42 | |
And you can see these tactors | 0:47:42 | 0:47:43 | |
giving information as to which way he is drifting in space. | 0:47:43 | 0:47:48 | |
So confident are they in how the system works, it's being put to the test | 0:47:48 | 0:47:52 | |
on a pilot who will attempt to fly with his eyes completely closed. | 0:47:52 | 0:47:58 | |
So it's at some risk that we are not successful | 0:48:00 | 0:48:04 | |
but that's the essence of science, is to experiment | 0:48:04 | 0:48:08 | |
and so this is raw... | 0:48:08 | 0:48:10 | |
and un-attempted-before footage. | 0:48:10 | 0:48:13 | |
We will have Captain Wingate close his eyes, and use the tactile cues to land | 0:48:13 | 0:48:19 | |
the helicopter so he is going to be fully reliant on feel for spatial orientation. | 0:48:19 | 0:48:26 | |
I would like you to take off down the runway. | 0:48:28 | 0:48:31 | |
Do you feel the upper tactor fire? | 0:48:31 | 0:48:33 | |
It's your shoulder harness telling you that you're above 100 feet, which is perfect. | 0:48:33 | 0:48:37 | |
Eyes closed. | 0:48:41 | 0:48:43 | |
Zero the tactor out on the belly button so you know you need a little bit of... | 0:48:43 | 0:48:47 | |
There is your belly button tactor. Keep your eyes closed. | 0:48:47 | 0:48:49 | |
Feel that increase, it means velocity is getting fast. | 0:48:49 | 0:48:54 | |
Slow it down a little bit. Very nice. | 0:48:54 | 0:48:57 | |
Don't dump power. You are on a nice descent right now. | 0:48:57 | 0:49:00 | |
I am not going to give you any warnings. | 0:49:01 | 0:49:04 | |
Your seat pad will tell you 10 ft. | 0:49:04 | 0:49:06 | |
That's not going around. That is me preparing cushion. | 0:49:06 | 0:49:10 | |
Right? You are on the ground. | 0:49:10 | 0:49:14 | |
LAUGHTER Nice job! Nice job! | 0:49:14 | 0:49:16 | |
That was a first for TSAS right there. | 0:49:18 | 0:49:21 | |
Eyes closed approach from over 100ft. | 0:49:21 | 0:49:25 | |
Straight to the ground | 0:49:25 | 0:49:27 | |
like crazy men. OK. | 0:49:27 | 0:49:30 | |
For pilots on the front lines, this ability to make more of their | 0:49:30 | 0:49:35 | |
other senses could make all the difference in the world. | 0:49:35 | 0:49:38 | |
I've done numerous dust landings sat roadsides where you have | 0:49:40 | 0:49:43 | |
zero reference with the ground because of the dust outside. | 0:49:43 | 0:49:48 | |
It's talcum powder - very thick and it envelops you. | 0:49:48 | 0:49:51 | |
So to be able to use your body and adjust appropriately, | 0:49:51 | 0:49:57 | |
not only are you saving the guy's life on the ground | 0:49:57 | 0:49:59 | |
but you also have the guys on board you are trying to protect. | 0:49:59 | 0:50:04 | |
It gives you another ability to... | 0:50:04 | 0:50:07 | |
adjust appropriately so that's amazing. | 0:50:07 | 0:50:09 | |
I am very excited and pleased to be able to say it is a wonderful feeling | 0:50:15 | 0:50:19 | |
in your heart to know you have an answer for a problem that will save | 0:50:19 | 0:50:23 | |
many lives under many different types of conditions, not just in aviation but in many other situations. | 0:50:23 | 0:50:29 | |
This new technology has profound implications, | 0:50:32 | 0:50:38 | |
helping to reveal what our senses are really capable of. | 0:50:38 | 0:50:42 | |
As we use touch, we will find there are more | 0:50:42 | 0:50:45 | |
and more applications in the future and they will be almost limitless, only limited by our imagination. | 0:50:45 | 0:50:52 | |
And it is up to us to come up with new and better ways to use this. | 0:50:52 | 0:50:57 | |
And I am sure there are people out there that will take this technology and carry it well into the future. | 0:50:57 | 0:51:02 | |
And that future may be just around the corner. | 0:51:04 | 0:51:08 | |
In the small German town of Osnabruck, a group of scientists | 0:51:22 | 0:51:26 | |
have been pioneering a groundbreaking new experiment. | 0:51:26 | 0:51:30 | |
They've been pushing the boundary of our sensory capability, attempting to give a man an entirely new sense. | 0:51:30 | 0:51:38 | |
They've been trying to see if humans can make use of the earth's magnetic field, just like birds. | 0:51:41 | 0:51:48 | |
So in the beginning we came up with the idea you | 0:51:49 | 0:51:52 | |
could use the magnetic field of the earth to augment the sensory system | 0:51:52 | 0:51:57 | |
and extend the sensory experience | 0:51:57 | 0:51:59 | |
you usually have. | 0:51:59 | 0:52:01 | |
For the past six years, they've been developing the feelSpace belt - | 0:52:01 | 0:52:06 | |
a vibrating sensory device that enables the wearer to feel the position of magnetic north. | 0:52:08 | 0:52:14 | |
So actually that's the prototype of the belt, so we did the first... | 0:52:16 | 0:52:21 | |
exploratory study with that belt | 0:52:21 | 0:52:23 | |
and it exists, basically | 0:52:23 | 0:52:26 | |
of a row of vibrators like in cell phones so these green things are vibrating. | 0:52:26 | 0:52:32 | |
And at the other side is the most important part. | 0:52:32 | 0:52:35 | |
The compass feeds information to the control box and the control box then controls | 0:52:35 | 0:52:42 | |
all these vibrators. | 0:52:42 | 0:52:44 | |
So if you put it around your waist, like this, there is always one of the vibrators vibrating. | 0:52:44 | 0:52:52 | |
This one is vibrating because there's north and if I turn like this, | 0:52:52 | 0:52:56 | |
the next one is vibrating and if I turn like this, the next one is vibrating. | 0:52:56 | 0:53:01 | |
So a signal's going around my waist. | 0:53:01 | 0:53:04 | |
I think it's not so important how it looks but it really works so...! | 0:53:04 | 0:53:08 | |
Udo Wachter was one of the volunteers who took part in the study. | 0:53:13 | 0:53:18 | |
For six weird weeks, he wore the belt every moment of his waking day. | 0:53:18 | 0:53:24 | |
In the beginning it was a little bit strange because one isn't very | 0:53:24 | 0:53:29 | |
used to having a constant buzzing on the body, | 0:53:29 | 0:53:33 | |
and I'm also a little bit ticklish in certain places! | 0:53:33 | 0:53:37 | |
But it didn't take very long to get used to it, | 0:53:38 | 0:53:42 | |
after a day or so, I didn't really realise it was there any more. | 0:53:42 | 0:53:47 | |
The first clue the team were on to something came when they noticed an important and unexpected phenomenon. | 0:53:50 | 0:53:57 | |
Strangely, wearers found it difficult to articulate what they were experiencing. | 0:53:59 | 0:54:04 | |
It was a sign something really significant was taking place. | 0:54:07 | 0:54:11 | |
It's a characteristic of senses that it's so specific and so special | 0:54:12 | 0:54:16 | |
that it's hard to communicate to someone who does not have it. | 0:54:16 | 0:54:20 | |
You can't communicate how it is to see red to someone who has never seen red. | 0:54:20 | 0:54:24 | |
It hints at the possibility that there is really integration of new sensory information going on. | 0:54:29 | 0:54:34 | |
In the modern world, there's no shortage of technology to find our way around. | 0:54:34 | 0:54:40 | |
But the feelSpace system was unlike any other kind of device. | 0:54:40 | 0:54:44 | |
For the first time it suggested new sensory | 0:54:47 | 0:54:49 | |
information could be absorbed without having to think about it. | 0:54:49 | 0:54:54 | |
You might say, you can just have a compass, and look at it and then you | 0:54:56 | 0:54:59 | |
can find your way anyways but this is what we do not want to find. | 0:54:59 | 0:55:05 | |
We want to help subjects integrate this kind of | 0:55:05 | 0:55:09 | |
information in a way which makes it available to them just intuitively. | 0:55:09 | 0:55:13 | |
Usually senses do not work or they do not need attention so you open your eyes and see, | 0:55:13 | 0:55:19 | |
you take out earplugs and you hear, and you just put your hand down here and you feel that it's sand here. | 0:55:19 | 0:55:26 | |
After six weeks of intensive training, | 0:55:31 | 0:55:34 | |
Udo and the other volunteers faced the ultimate challenge. | 0:55:34 | 0:55:38 | |
Using only his new magnetic sense, Udo had to navigate blindfold around a previously unseen shape. | 0:55:40 | 0:55:48 | |
And even when deliberately disorientated, | 0:55:58 | 0:56:02 | |
he still managed to find his way back to his starting position with remarkable accuracy... | 0:56:03 | 0:56:10 | |
an otherwise impossible task. | 0:56:12 | 0:56:15 | |
While wearing the belt, it felt like having a new sense and after a very short time it just felt like it | 0:56:17 | 0:56:27 | |
should always be there, and it felt like it always was there. | 0:56:27 | 0:56:31 | |
After its initial trial, the feelSpace belt offers us a glimpse of the future, | 0:56:33 | 0:56:38 | |
suggesting we may not be limited by the senses we are born with. | 0:56:38 | 0:56:44 | |
The team are already working on a more extensive trial, | 0:56:44 | 0:56:48 | |
where they will probe the system's impact on the brain in even greater detail | 0:56:48 | 0:56:54 | |
but it's clear that when it comes to creating new senses, this is just the beginning. | 0:56:54 | 0:57:01 | |
Just the idea that there are more ways | 0:57:01 | 0:57:04 | |
to experience the world is just fascinating. | 0:57:04 | 0:57:07 | |
I wouldn't say it's going beyond... | 0:57:07 | 0:57:09 | |
er... | 0:57:09 | 0:57:11 | |
evolution, it's more like... | 0:57:11 | 0:57:14 | |
I would say it's more like a part of evolution. | 0:57:14 | 0:57:18 | |
Because I would say evolution is nothing which stopped ten years ago, | 0:57:18 | 0:57:23 | |
or 100 years ago, or stopped just now, | 0:57:23 | 0:57:25 | |
but it's going on. And if it's going to work out and if we are successful with this study and we find out it is | 0:57:25 | 0:57:33 | |
working then it's an important step for science, but also for everyone who is a human being, in a way. | 0:57:33 | 0:57:41 | |
Over the past ten years, our understanding of the senses has undergone a revolution. | 0:57:47 | 0:57:54 | |
It's enabling us to finally unlock the extraordinary potential of our minds... | 0:57:56 | 0:58:01 | |
and promising to transform all our lives in the most weird and wonderful ways. | 0:58:05 | 0:58:11 | |
So next time you're not sure whether to believe what you see... | 0:58:16 | 0:58:19 | |
enjoy it, because these tricks of the mind are how you make sense of your world. | 0:58:21 | 0:58:29 | |
Subtitles by Red Bee Media Ltd | 0:58:43 | 0:58:46 | |
E-mail [email protected] | 0:58:46 | 0:58:48 |