Browse content similar to Silent Messages. Check below for episodes and series from the same categories and more!
Line | From | To | |
---|---|---|---|
I use my mobile phone to send silent messages all the time. | 0:00:04 | 0:00:09 | |
But what if I could control someone else's face? | 0:00:09 | 0:00:12 | |
I'm sending quite a message there, aren't I? | 0:00:14 | 0:00:16 | |
That looked a bit surprised. | 0:00:16 | 0:00:17 | |
Now, our faces are extraordinarily powerful tools | 0:00:17 | 0:00:21 | |
for silent communication. | 0:00:21 | 0:00:23 | |
We can say a huge amount | 0:00:23 | 0:00:25 | |
without using any sound or any speech at all. | 0:00:25 | 0:00:29 | |
In this lecture, we're going to reveal | 0:00:29 | 0:00:31 | |
why silent messages are one of the | 0:00:31 | 0:00:33 | |
most powerful forms of communication on the planet, | 0:00:33 | 0:00:36 | |
and why, for many animals, | 0:00:36 | 0:00:38 | |
it can make the difference between life and death. | 0:00:38 | 0:00:41 | |
Welcome to the second of | 0:01:02 | 0:01:04 | |
the 2017 Royal Institution Christmas Lectures. | 0:01:04 | 0:01:07 | |
I'm Professor Sophie Scott. | 0:01:07 | 0:01:09 | |
Right now, I'm talking to you to get my message across with my voice. | 0:01:09 | 0:01:13 | |
But like you just saw with my colleague's twitching face, | 0:01:13 | 0:01:16 | |
not all communications are made with sounds. | 0:01:16 | 0:01:19 | |
We, like many other animals, | 0:01:19 | 0:01:21 | |
use our bodies to send silent messages | 0:01:21 | 0:01:23 | |
about who we are, how we feel, | 0:01:23 | 0:01:26 | |
what our intentions might really be. | 0:01:26 | 0:01:28 | |
In this lecture, | 0:01:28 | 0:01:30 | |
I'm going to give you an insight | 0:01:30 | 0:01:31 | |
into this world of silent communication, | 0:01:31 | 0:01:34 | |
and reveal how you can start to spot and decipher some of it. | 0:01:34 | 0:01:38 | |
I'm going to start with what is probably | 0:01:40 | 0:01:42 | |
the first way that any animals | 0:01:42 | 0:01:43 | |
worked out how to communicate. | 0:01:43 | 0:01:45 | |
That's using chemical messages. | 0:01:45 | 0:01:47 | |
We would call those smells. | 0:01:47 | 0:01:49 | |
I'm going to send your noses some information and you're going to know | 0:01:49 | 0:01:54 | |
when that information's coming towards you | 0:01:54 | 0:01:57 | |
because you'll see a smoke ring. | 0:01:57 | 0:01:58 | |
Anybody tell me what that smelt like? | 0:02:16 | 0:02:18 | |
There's something kind of sweet there, isn't there? | 0:02:18 | 0:02:20 | |
There is a sort of sweet smell. | 0:02:20 | 0:02:21 | |
I think it's supposed to be kind of like a | 0:02:21 | 0:02:23 | |
candyfloss kind of smell, OK? | 0:02:23 | 0:02:25 | |
That might suggest something edible, | 0:02:25 | 0:02:27 | |
something edible's going on. | 0:02:27 | 0:02:28 | |
Let's try a different smell. | 0:02:28 | 0:02:31 | |
Ha-ha. | 0:02:36 | 0:02:37 | |
You took a direct hit there. | 0:02:44 | 0:02:47 | |
What do you think about that smell? | 0:02:47 | 0:02:50 | |
Uh, poo. | 0:02:50 | 0:02:52 | |
Do you like it? | 0:02:52 | 0:02:54 | |
Yeah, it's great! | 0:02:54 | 0:02:56 | |
I quite like it, | 0:02:56 | 0:02:57 | |
it reminds me of home! | 0:02:57 | 0:02:59 | |
Now, that was, indeed, the smell of poo. | 0:03:00 | 0:03:02 | |
It's called skatole and it's a synthesised version of some of the | 0:03:02 | 0:03:05 | |
molecules in poo and probably if you were to smell that, you might think, | 0:03:05 | 0:03:09 | |
"I don't know if I do want to eat that food." | 0:03:09 | 0:03:11 | |
You know, it's contaminated, there's information there. | 0:03:11 | 0:03:14 | |
Now, slightly disturbingly, | 0:03:14 | 0:03:16 | |
all smells are made of something physical. | 0:03:16 | 0:03:19 | |
They are chemical molecules, they are literally parts of a thing, | 0:03:19 | 0:03:23 | |
a body or an object that have been released into the air, | 0:03:23 | 0:03:27 | |
and we detect those in our noses by chemical detectors. | 0:03:27 | 0:03:31 | |
Sending messages with chemicals like smells | 0:03:31 | 0:03:34 | |
is really a very basic form of communication. | 0:03:34 | 0:03:36 | |
And that's because everything, | 0:03:36 | 0:03:38 | |
from bacteria to blue whales, is made of chemicals. | 0:03:38 | 0:03:42 | |
So, the simplest way to send a message is | 0:03:42 | 0:03:44 | |
just leave some of those chemicals behind. | 0:03:44 | 0:03:48 | |
A chemical that an organism uses to communicate | 0:03:48 | 0:03:50 | |
with another member of the | 0:03:50 | 0:03:51 | |
same species is called a pheromone, | 0:03:51 | 0:03:54 | |
and this is any chemical that's released by one individual to affect | 0:03:54 | 0:03:57 | |
another individual's behaviour. | 0:03:57 | 0:04:00 | |
And amongst the animals, | 0:04:00 | 0:04:01 | |
the absolute champion users of pheromones are insects. | 0:04:01 | 0:04:05 | |
In fact, many insects rely | 0:04:05 | 0:04:06 | |
completely on using smells to communicate with each other. | 0:04:06 | 0:04:10 | |
To find out more, | 0:04:10 | 0:04:12 | |
please welcome an expert in how insects use pheromone messaging. | 0:04:12 | 0:04:16 | |
From Rothamsted Research, Dr Gia Aradottir. | 0:04:16 | 0:04:19 | |
-Hi. -Hello, lovely to meet you. | 0:04:25 | 0:04:26 | |
Nice to meet you. | 0:04:26 | 0:04:28 | |
Now, if you could just step forward. | 0:04:28 | 0:04:30 | |
Can you show us, what have you got in your hand there? | 0:04:30 | 0:04:33 | |
So, here I have some aphids on a leaf. | 0:04:33 | 0:04:36 | |
-OK. -They're very small, you won't be able to see them from there. | 0:04:36 | 0:04:39 | |
But if I put them under the microscope here, hopefully, | 0:04:39 | 0:04:43 | |
you can see them on the screen. | 0:04:43 | 0:04:44 | |
Yep. | 0:04:46 | 0:04:48 | |
Can you see these? So, these aphids are all mothers and daughters, | 0:04:48 | 0:04:51 | |
they're all female, all girls. | 0:04:51 | 0:04:53 | |
-Aphid power! -Aphid power. | 0:04:53 | 0:04:55 | |
And they're happily feeding on the phloem on the leaf. | 0:04:55 | 0:04:59 | |
OK. So, we've got a group of aphids. | 0:04:59 | 0:05:01 | |
If they wanted to send an alarm signal, | 0:05:01 | 0:05:03 | |
if something was going wrong, | 0:05:03 | 0:05:04 | |
what would they do? | 0:05:04 | 0:05:06 | |
If one of them is attacked by a predator, like a ladybird, | 0:05:06 | 0:05:09 | |
for example, it will send a message, an alarm pheromone, | 0:05:09 | 0:05:13 | |
out into the air, | 0:05:13 | 0:05:14 | |
and that basically tells all of the other ones that they need to escape. | 0:05:14 | 0:05:18 | |
-Can we see that? -Let's try. | 0:05:18 | 0:05:20 | |
So, I have here some aphid alarm pheromone in this little vial. | 0:05:20 | 0:05:25 | |
And if I put a tiny drop of alarm pheromone on the leaf next to the | 0:05:25 | 0:05:31 | |
aphids, then, hopefully... | 0:05:31 | 0:05:32 | |
Oh, they're off. | 0:05:38 | 0:05:39 | |
Can you see? They all move away | 0:05:41 | 0:05:42 | |
because they think that one of them is being attacked. | 0:05:42 | 0:05:45 | |
Can we smell that? | 0:05:45 | 0:05:48 | |
I can't. Let me put a little bit here on. | 0:05:48 | 0:05:52 | |
The aphids all ran away but we didn't! | 0:05:52 | 0:05:54 | |
No, I don't think I can smell that. This is one of the really | 0:05:57 | 0:05:59 | |
interesting things about smells, isn't it? | 0:05:59 | 0:06:01 | |
Because if you haven't got the | 0:06:01 | 0:06:03 | |
chemical receptors to detect a smell, | 0:06:03 | 0:06:05 | |
it's just not there, it's completely invisible to you. | 0:06:05 | 0:06:07 | |
You are blind to it. | 0:06:07 | 0:06:09 | |
So, this is a very private way of having a conversation. | 0:06:09 | 0:06:11 | |
It is. And it's happening in our gardens, in our parks, | 0:06:11 | 0:06:14 | |
and we have no idea what's going on. | 0:06:14 | 0:06:16 | |
But if some other animal could learn about that message, | 0:06:17 | 0:06:21 | |
could they start to intercept this information? | 0:06:21 | 0:06:24 | |
Yeah, nature is so clever. | 0:06:24 | 0:06:26 | |
So, predators have already learned | 0:06:26 | 0:06:28 | |
how to recognise the alarm pheromone of the aphid. | 0:06:28 | 0:06:32 | |
They are basically eavesdropping on | 0:06:32 | 0:06:33 | |
the communication between the aphids. | 0:06:33 | 0:06:36 | |
This alarm pheromone tells them that there is a prey there, | 0:06:36 | 0:06:39 | |
there's an aphid that they can go and eat, for example. | 0:06:39 | 0:06:42 | |
What kind of predator do we mean? | 0:06:42 | 0:06:44 | |
So, we have parasitic wasps, | 0:06:44 | 0:06:46 | |
and I've got some here in this little Petri dish. | 0:06:46 | 0:06:49 | |
Manage your anxiety, they're really small, | 0:06:49 | 0:06:51 | |
I don't think they'll go after us. | 0:06:51 | 0:06:53 | |
They're very, very small and they only go for aphids. | 0:06:53 | 0:06:55 | |
Let's see if we can find one. | 0:06:57 | 0:07:00 | |
So, the wasps are picking up on the communication between the aphids. | 0:07:00 | 0:07:04 | |
-They're listening in? -They are. | 0:07:04 | 0:07:05 | |
To the signals from the pheromones? | 0:07:05 | 0:07:07 | |
Yes. Absolutely. | 0:07:07 | 0:07:09 | |
Is that an aphid? That's an aphid. | 0:07:09 | 0:07:11 | |
Oh, dear, is it at terrible risk? | 0:07:11 | 0:07:13 | |
It might be. | 0:07:13 | 0:07:15 | |
We can look. | 0:07:15 | 0:07:16 | |
-Oh. -See? | 0:07:16 | 0:07:18 | |
Did you see that? | 0:07:18 | 0:07:21 | |
What was actually happening there? | 0:07:21 | 0:07:23 | |
I think it may actually have laid an egg inside the aphid. | 0:07:23 | 0:07:26 | |
-The wasp? -The wasp. | 0:07:26 | 0:07:27 | |
Laid an egg inside the aphid? | 0:07:27 | 0:07:29 | |
Yeah. That's what they do. They search for aphids, | 0:07:29 | 0:07:32 | |
and they lay their eggs inside the aphid. | 0:07:32 | 0:07:34 | |
The egg then hatches, and the larva eats the aphid from the inside out. | 0:07:34 | 0:07:39 | |
So, to be absolutely clear, we just saw something absolutely terrifying? | 0:07:39 | 0:07:43 | |
-I think so. -From the point of view of an aphid. | 0:07:43 | 0:07:45 | |
-Yeah, from the point of view of an aphid. -But great if you're the wasp. | 0:07:45 | 0:07:48 | |
-That's amazing. -That's fantastic. Yeah. Thank you very much, Gia. | 0:07:48 | 0:07:51 | |
Thank you so much. | 0:07:51 | 0:07:52 | |
So, how are these insects picking up on the chemical messages? | 0:07:58 | 0:08:02 | |
If you look at an aphid, | 0:08:02 | 0:08:04 | |
you can see it's got these large antler-like projections | 0:08:04 | 0:08:08 | |
coming out of the top of its head. | 0:08:08 | 0:08:10 | |
Those are its antennae. | 0:08:10 | 0:08:12 | |
They are amazing at picking up pheromones. | 0:08:12 | 0:08:15 | |
But how do they actually work? | 0:08:15 | 0:08:17 | |
If we were to zoom in to the surface of the antenna, | 0:08:17 | 0:08:22 | |
what we find is they're covered in these fine hairs, sensory hairs, | 0:08:22 | 0:08:26 | |
and the hairs are what is actually doing the smelling. | 0:08:26 | 0:08:29 | |
The hairs have got very small holes in them. | 0:08:29 | 0:08:32 | |
And these holes let molecules of air and other chemicals come through. | 0:08:32 | 0:08:36 | |
So, that's moving through all the time, | 0:08:36 | 0:08:38 | |
chemicals are moving in and moving out. | 0:08:38 | 0:08:40 | |
In the centre of the hair... | 0:08:43 | 0:08:45 | |
..there is a nerve cell. | 0:08:46 | 0:08:48 | |
And what the nerve cell wants to do is detect pheromones. | 0:08:48 | 0:08:51 | |
It's got the receptors for the pheromones. | 0:08:51 | 0:08:53 | |
But if stuff's just moving through the holes, | 0:08:53 | 0:08:57 | |
it doesn't make contact with this nerve cell. | 0:08:57 | 0:09:01 | |
What we need is another factor, | 0:09:01 | 0:09:04 | |
something to actually link it all together and that's called an | 0:09:04 | 0:09:06 | |
odour-binding protein. | 0:09:06 | 0:09:09 | |
And I need a volunteer to help me with this demonstration. | 0:09:09 | 0:09:12 | |
Ideally, I need an odour-binding protein, | 0:09:12 | 0:09:15 | |
but I will also accept a human! | 0:09:15 | 0:09:18 | |
Can I have you? Thank you very much. | 0:09:18 | 0:09:20 | |
You look like a very excellent odour-binding protein! | 0:09:20 | 0:09:25 | |
Thank you. | 0:09:25 | 0:09:27 | |
Now, what's your name? | 0:09:28 | 0:09:30 | |
-Shreya. -Shreya? | 0:09:30 | 0:09:32 | |
-Yes. -Now, Shreya, you're no longer Shreya, | 0:09:32 | 0:09:34 | |
you are my odour-binding protein. | 0:09:34 | 0:09:37 | |
And to convey this, you need to wear the odour-binding protein hat. | 0:09:37 | 0:09:41 | |
I'm sorry, it's just the rule for the odour-binding protein club, | 0:09:42 | 0:09:45 | |
come here, hat on. | 0:09:45 | 0:09:46 | |
Transformation, I think you'll agree. | 0:09:48 | 0:09:50 | |
And I need you to take | 0:09:50 | 0:09:53 | |
your odour-binding protein receptor and put that on | 0:09:53 | 0:09:56 | |
your right hand. There you go. | 0:09:56 | 0:09:57 | |
Now, that's what you're going to use to catch molecules. | 0:09:57 | 0:10:00 | |
But you can't catch all the chemicals. | 0:10:00 | 0:10:04 | |
You can only catch things that will work | 0:10:04 | 0:10:06 | |
with the odours you have the receptor for. | 0:10:06 | 0:10:08 | |
So, I'm going to try this. | 0:10:08 | 0:10:09 | |
Will you be able to catch this one? | 0:10:09 | 0:10:11 | |
No. | 0:10:13 | 0:10:14 | |
Oh. Excellent, mad skills, turn it upside down. | 0:10:16 | 0:10:19 | |
There we go. Now, you really can catch that. | 0:10:19 | 0:10:21 | |
So, what I want you to do is, they're going to start moving at | 0:10:21 | 0:10:24 | |
speed through these holes in the hair. | 0:10:24 | 0:10:26 | |
What I want you to do is catch them and, when you've caught one, | 0:10:26 | 0:10:30 | |
bring it over to the nerve cell and make a contact. | 0:10:30 | 0:10:33 | |
You also need to make a contact with your hand as well so you've | 0:10:33 | 0:10:35 | |
completely triggered the nerve cell. | 0:10:35 | 0:10:37 | |
And that's sending the message off to the aphid's brain. | 0:10:37 | 0:10:39 | |
When you've finished, just drop the ball in there, and you're done, OK? | 0:10:39 | 0:10:42 | |
Are you ready? We're going to see | 0:10:42 | 0:10:44 | |
how many of these pheromones you can | 0:10:44 | 0:10:46 | |
catch in the next 20 seconds. | 0:10:46 | 0:10:49 | |
I'm going to count you in. | 0:10:49 | 0:10:50 | |
Three, two, one. | 0:10:51 | 0:10:55 | |
There you go, there you go, other hand. | 0:10:58 | 0:11:00 | |
Good, brilliant. Pop it in there. Fantastic. | 0:11:00 | 0:11:03 | |
You can catch them on the floor. You can catch them on the floor. | 0:11:08 | 0:11:11 | |
-AUDIENCE: -Five, four, three, two, one. | 0:11:11 | 0:11:16 | |
Well done! | 0:11:16 | 0:11:17 | |
Well done, Shreya. | 0:11:20 | 0:11:22 | |
Well done. | 0:11:22 | 0:11:25 | |
That was very good. You got one but I don't think I'd have caught any. | 0:11:25 | 0:11:27 | |
So, thank you very much. I'll have your... | 0:11:27 | 0:11:29 | |
I'll transform you back into a human. | 0:11:29 | 0:11:31 | |
So, what you did there was you were actually allowing the aphid to know | 0:11:31 | 0:11:36 | |
that there was an aphid alarm pheromone | 0:11:36 | 0:11:38 | |
in the environment by actually causing that nerve cell to fire. | 0:11:38 | 0:11:41 | |
So, your aphid has safely escaped | 0:11:41 | 0:11:42 | |
and you can all run out of the room now, if you wish to! There we go. | 0:11:42 | 0:11:46 | |
Thank you very much, Shreya, thank you. | 0:11:46 | 0:11:48 | |
You can think of pheromones as being a bit like hormones, | 0:11:55 | 0:11:57 | |
they're things that affect... | 0:11:57 | 0:11:59 | |
Chemicals that affect your body and your behaviour. | 0:11:59 | 0:12:01 | |
But unlike hormones, | 0:12:01 | 0:12:03 | |
it's a signal that's coming from outside of the body. | 0:12:03 | 0:12:06 | |
Insects live in this world where pheromones are extremely important. | 0:12:06 | 0:12:11 | |
But what about animals closer to us in the evolutionary tree? | 0:12:11 | 0:12:14 | |
Do they use pheromones as well? | 0:12:14 | 0:12:17 | |
Well, here is an animal that's not necessarily | 0:12:17 | 0:12:20 | |
absolutely everybody's favourite animal. | 0:12:20 | 0:12:23 | |
Snakes. | 0:12:23 | 0:12:24 | |
Now, Phil, I see that you've got two snakes. | 0:12:30 | 0:12:35 | |
I've got one. I need some volunteers. | 0:12:35 | 0:12:38 | |
I need a volunteer who's absolutely | 0:12:38 | 0:12:40 | |
not going to freak out when and if | 0:12:40 | 0:12:42 | |
they meet a snake. OK. | 0:12:42 | 0:12:44 | |
Can I have... | 0:12:44 | 0:12:46 | |
You there in the pink sweater? You there, yes. | 0:12:46 | 0:12:49 | |
And can I have you there in the Christmas sweater with the glasses? | 0:12:49 | 0:12:53 | |
Yeah, fantastic. | 0:12:53 | 0:12:54 | |
I don't see why I should be the only person freaking out because they've | 0:12:57 | 0:13:00 | |
got a snake going down their blouse. | 0:13:00 | 0:13:02 | |
Right, OK. So, what's your name? | 0:13:02 | 0:13:04 | |
Have your hand up like that... | 0:13:04 | 0:13:06 | |
-Obiay. -Obiay? Hello, Obiay. And so, what's your name? | 0:13:06 | 0:13:08 | |
-Gia. -Gia. Now, Gia. -Well done. -Oh, thank you. | 0:13:08 | 0:13:12 | |
Can you see these snakes are sticking their tongues out a lot? | 0:13:12 | 0:13:16 | |
That's because this is an unfamiliar environment for these snakes and | 0:13:16 | 0:13:19 | |
they're exploring it, they're exploring it with their tongues. | 0:13:19 | 0:13:22 | |
The really interesting part about these tongues | 0:13:22 | 0:13:26 | |
is that they themselves don't have any receptors on them. | 0:13:26 | 0:13:30 | |
They haven't got taste buds on, like our tongues. | 0:13:30 | 0:13:32 | |
What the snakes are doing, | 0:13:32 | 0:13:34 | |
they're capturing molecules from the air and they're bringing them into | 0:13:34 | 0:13:38 | |
the snakes' mouths. Now, this is different from the snake's nose. | 0:13:38 | 0:13:41 | |
You can see they've got nostrils up at the top of their face. | 0:13:41 | 0:13:44 | |
The organ that they've got inside their mouths | 0:13:44 | 0:13:47 | |
is called a vomeronasal organ. | 0:13:47 | 0:13:49 | |
And it actually lives... | 0:13:49 | 0:13:51 | |
..on the roof of the snake's mouth. | 0:13:55 | 0:13:58 | |
So, they bring the tongue in, | 0:13:58 | 0:14:01 | |
and they dip the two tips of the tongue into each side, | 0:14:01 | 0:14:06 | |
there's actually two little holes for the vomeronasal organ, | 0:14:06 | 0:14:09 | |
and that's making direct contact with pheromones into nerve cells. | 0:14:09 | 0:14:14 | |
So, that information can pass straight up into the snake's brain. | 0:14:14 | 0:14:18 | |
There's a fork in the snake's tongue and that means they can tell if the | 0:14:20 | 0:14:24 | |
smells have come from the left or the right. | 0:14:24 | 0:14:26 | |
So, it's able to move in the direction | 0:14:26 | 0:14:28 | |
that the pheromone has come from. | 0:14:28 | 0:14:30 | |
What's all this for? | 0:14:32 | 0:14:34 | |
Well, mostly, for snakes, it's about finding a mate. | 0:14:34 | 0:14:37 | |
Snakes are shy creatures that hide and ambush their prey. | 0:14:37 | 0:14:41 | |
And this system means that the male snakes can detect pheromone messages | 0:14:41 | 0:14:45 | |
being given out by a receptive female snake from a great distance. | 0:14:45 | 0:14:49 | |
Good news for any lady snakes in here this evening. | 0:14:49 | 0:14:52 | |
So, I'm going to say bye-bye to our highly friendly snakes. | 0:14:52 | 0:14:57 | |
That's fine, you guys, well done. | 0:14:57 | 0:14:59 | |
-Well done. -Obiay, fantastic. | 0:15:00 | 0:15:02 | |
Gia, very brave. | 0:15:02 | 0:15:04 | |
-Well done. -Fantastic, thank you. | 0:15:04 | 0:15:06 | |
They did brilliantly. | 0:15:11 | 0:15:14 | |
I was freaking out, I don't know if you could tell that. | 0:15:14 | 0:15:16 | |
So, what other animals also have a vomeronasal organ? | 0:15:16 | 0:15:21 | |
Well, it turns out quite a lot of them do. | 0:15:21 | 0:15:23 | |
A lot of other mammals have got vomeronasal organs that they use for | 0:15:23 | 0:15:27 | |
smelling pheromones. | 0:15:27 | 0:15:28 | |
So, if you've ever looked at dogs | 0:15:28 | 0:15:30 | |
smelling other dogs' bottoms and thought, | 0:15:30 | 0:15:33 | |
"I fail to see the appeal of that," | 0:15:33 | 0:15:35 | |
well, actually, it's because the dogs are smelling pheromones. | 0:15:35 | 0:15:38 | |
And they are very short-lived pheromones, | 0:15:38 | 0:15:40 | |
so they can't pass off into the air, | 0:15:40 | 0:15:42 | |
so the dog's really got to put its nose | 0:15:42 | 0:15:44 | |
into its friend's bottom to get to those pheromones. | 0:15:44 | 0:15:46 | |
Probably too much information there! | 0:15:46 | 0:15:48 | |
When cats pull a funny face, | 0:15:48 | 0:15:50 | |
if a cat's ever got on your lap and gone... | 0:15:50 | 0:15:53 | |
..that's the cat trying to smell your pheromones. | 0:15:54 | 0:15:57 | |
Now, I love cats. | 0:15:57 | 0:15:59 | |
I don't know if I absolutely want them smelling what they might think | 0:15:59 | 0:16:02 | |
might be my pheromones, but it's what they do. | 0:16:02 | 0:16:05 | |
It's because it matters so much to mammals. | 0:16:05 | 0:16:07 | |
They live in a world of smell. | 0:16:07 | 0:16:09 | |
Do humans - we're mammals - do we have a vomeronasal organ? | 0:16:09 | 0:16:12 | |
Can we use smell to send messages? | 0:16:12 | 0:16:14 | |
Well, if we look at the roof of a human mouth... | 0:16:16 | 0:16:20 | |
..what you find is no evidence of a vomeronasal organ. | 0:16:21 | 0:16:27 | |
We don't seem to have the receptors | 0:16:27 | 0:16:30 | |
to actually pick up pheromone information. | 0:16:30 | 0:16:34 | |
Now, we like smells, don't we? | 0:16:35 | 0:16:38 | |
We do. We pay attention to smells, | 0:16:38 | 0:16:39 | |
you cared about the smells we sent | 0:16:39 | 0:16:41 | |
around the lecture theatre earlier in the evening, | 0:16:41 | 0:16:43 | |
but they are less important to us than they are to other animals. | 0:16:43 | 0:16:48 | |
For other mammals, smell dominates. | 0:16:48 | 0:16:50 | |
It can be the most important sense. | 0:16:50 | 0:16:52 | |
Why might we have lost some of our ancestors' smelling abilities, | 0:16:54 | 0:16:59 | |
and along with it maybe our ability to use pheromones? | 0:16:59 | 0:17:03 | |
Well, to think about that, we need to think about how primates evolved. | 0:17:03 | 0:17:07 | |
These are three primate skulls. | 0:17:10 | 0:17:14 | |
We look here, this is an ancient primate, this is a lemur. | 0:17:14 | 0:17:18 | |
That's an ancestral ape, so that's one of the ancestors of apes. | 0:17:18 | 0:17:22 | |
And this is a modern human. | 0:17:22 | 0:17:24 | |
And one of the things you can notice | 0:17:24 | 0:17:26 | |
as you get to humans is that faces... | 0:17:26 | 0:17:28 | |
..start to get much flatter. | 0:17:31 | 0:17:33 | |
We just have smaller noses. | 0:17:33 | 0:17:35 | |
We've got less space for there to be smell information detected. | 0:17:35 | 0:17:39 | |
Smell is just downplayed right from the start in humans. | 0:17:39 | 0:17:43 | |
And you can also see that there have been other changes. | 0:17:43 | 0:17:46 | |
So, for example, our faces are longer, our mouths are taller, | 0:17:46 | 0:17:50 | |
there's more space inside our mouths, | 0:17:50 | 0:17:52 | |
and we have much bigger eyes. | 0:17:52 | 0:17:54 | |
And, in fact, if you look in the brain, you see a similar pattern. | 0:17:54 | 0:17:58 | |
A lot of other mammals have got huge amounts | 0:17:58 | 0:18:00 | |
of their brain devoted to dealing with smell. | 0:18:00 | 0:18:03 | |
In humans, those smell areas are very small. | 0:18:03 | 0:18:06 | |
And, in contrast, in humans, | 0:18:06 | 0:18:07 | |
the brain areas associated with processing | 0:18:07 | 0:18:10 | |
sound and vision are much larger. | 0:18:10 | 0:18:11 | |
So, what we've done is we've really | 0:18:11 | 0:18:15 | |
traded off the space in our faces and in | 0:18:15 | 0:18:19 | |
our skulls for dealing with smell information, | 0:18:19 | 0:18:22 | |
and we've replaced it with vision and hearing. | 0:18:22 | 0:18:25 | |
And that's probably because... | 0:18:25 | 0:18:27 | |
..vision and hearing have great advantages for humans, | 0:18:28 | 0:18:31 | |
not least for communication. | 0:18:31 | 0:18:33 | |
Now, no disrespect to smell, smell works really well for communication. | 0:18:35 | 0:18:39 | |
At a distance, for example, smell can be great. | 0:18:39 | 0:18:42 | |
You saw that with the snakes. | 0:18:42 | 0:18:44 | |
But smells will only move at the speed of the air around them, | 0:18:44 | 0:18:47 | |
and they can linger a long while. | 0:18:47 | 0:18:48 | |
I am sure the aphids would prefer | 0:18:48 | 0:18:50 | |
that the wasps couldn't pick up on their | 0:18:50 | 0:18:52 | |
distant alarm pheromone messages. | 0:18:52 | 0:18:55 | |
If, however, you want to send faster, more complicated messages, | 0:18:55 | 0:18:59 | |
you need a speedier system. | 0:18:59 | 0:19:02 | |
So, let's look at a more flexible way to communicate. | 0:19:02 | 0:19:05 | |
Let there be light. | 0:19:07 | 0:19:08 | |
Many animals produce their own light. | 0:19:11 | 0:19:13 | |
This helix of light is made in a | 0:19:13 | 0:19:15 | |
very similar way by mixing together two chemicals. | 0:19:15 | 0:19:17 | |
In the natural world, | 0:19:17 | 0:19:19 | |
a chemical called luciferin is | 0:19:19 | 0:19:21 | |
mixed with an enzyme called luciferase. | 0:19:21 | 0:19:24 | |
The reaction causes light to be given off. | 0:19:24 | 0:19:27 | |
-AUDIENCE: -Ooh! | 0:19:27 | 0:19:30 | |
Making light via this kind of chemical reaction | 0:19:30 | 0:19:33 | |
is actually one of the | 0:19:33 | 0:19:34 | |
commonest methods of communication on Earth. | 0:19:34 | 0:19:38 | |
It's called bioluminescence, and deep sea fish are famous for it. | 0:19:38 | 0:19:43 | |
To explain more, please welcome from the Natural History Museum, | 0:19:43 | 0:19:46 | |
curator of fish, James Maclaine! | 0:19:46 | 0:19:49 | |
Hello, James. Nice to meet you. | 0:19:54 | 0:19:57 | |
Now, if you could just tell me | 0:19:57 | 0:19:59 | |
what you've brought along for us today? | 0:19:59 | 0:20:01 | |
I've brought along a tiny fraction of our huge deep sea fish collection | 0:20:01 | 0:20:05 | |
that we have at the Natural History Museum. | 0:20:05 | 0:20:07 | |
So, in our first jar we have two specimens of a thing | 0:20:07 | 0:20:11 | |
called a Threadthin dragonfish. | 0:20:11 | 0:20:13 | |
So it has a light organ | 0:20:13 | 0:20:15 | |
on a little barb underneath its chin, | 0:20:15 | 0:20:18 | |
and it uses that to attract its prey towards its mouth. | 0:20:18 | 0:20:21 | |
And then all along its tummy it has rows and rows | 0:20:21 | 0:20:24 | |
of little lights there. | 0:20:24 | 0:20:25 | |
And what that's for is actually for camouflaging itself, | 0:20:25 | 0:20:28 | |
so during the day when there is still a very faint amount of light | 0:20:28 | 0:20:31 | |
above, it makes its underside glow very faint blue, | 0:20:31 | 0:20:34 | |
so that if you look up, you can't see it. | 0:20:34 | 0:20:36 | |
And then, my favourite thing about them, | 0:20:36 | 0:20:38 | |
is they have a light organ behind their eye, | 0:20:38 | 0:20:40 | |
and in the male it's much, | 0:20:40 | 0:20:42 | |
much bigger than in the female. | 0:20:42 | 0:20:44 | |
So what we think is, it's like | 0:20:44 | 0:20:45 | |
a sort of deep sea version of a peacock's tail. | 0:20:45 | 0:20:47 | |
It's a display thing, so it gets her attention. | 0:20:47 | 0:20:50 | |
Fantastic. What about these guys? | 0:20:50 | 0:20:52 | |
They are called Lanternfish, | 0:20:52 | 0:20:53 | |
and they have a very distinctive pattern of lights along their sides. | 0:20:53 | 0:20:56 | |
It's a bit like a constellation of stars. | 0:20:56 | 0:20:58 | |
It's very distinctive. And it's so | 0:20:58 | 0:21:00 | |
distinctive that's what they use to recognise each other. | 0:21:00 | 0:21:02 | |
They live in big shoals, | 0:21:02 | 0:21:04 | |
so that's how they work out they are | 0:21:04 | 0:21:05 | |
-in a shoal of the same Lanternfish as themselves. -Amazing. | 0:21:05 | 0:21:09 | |
-What about this guy? -This one is quite interesting. | 0:21:09 | 0:21:11 | |
This is a stoplight loosejaw. | 0:21:11 | 0:21:14 | |
And everything I've mentioned so far is using blue light, | 0:21:14 | 0:21:16 | |
but this uses blue lights and red lights. | 0:21:16 | 0:21:19 | |
Why do they use blue lights? | 0:21:19 | 0:21:20 | |
Blue light travels the furthest. | 0:21:20 | 0:21:21 | |
It's the frequency of light that penetrates the furthest, | 0:21:21 | 0:21:24 | |
so if you want to get your signal out there, blue light is | 0:21:24 | 0:21:27 | |
the best light colour to use. | 0:21:27 | 0:21:29 | |
-OK. -But this | 0:21:29 | 0:21:31 | |
can also create red light. | 0:21:31 | 0:21:32 | |
It has, like, a light organ just | 0:21:32 | 0:21:34 | |
underneath its eye, but points forward, like a torch beam. | 0:21:34 | 0:21:37 | |
It can hunt out little animals using its red light, | 0:21:37 | 0:21:40 | |
and nothing else can see it, because | 0:21:40 | 0:21:41 | |
everything is tuned to the blue light. | 0:21:41 | 0:21:43 | |
And, finally, what do we have here? | 0:21:43 | 0:21:45 | |
I think this is my favourite. | 0:21:45 | 0:21:46 | |
I like it so much, I'm going to get it out for you. | 0:21:46 | 0:21:49 | |
There is actually two fish in here. This is a deep sea anglerfish. | 0:21:49 | 0:21:51 | |
The males and the females are very different. | 0:21:51 | 0:21:53 | |
The males have become very, very small. | 0:21:53 | 0:21:56 | |
They are now so small and pathetic that they can't actually survive by | 0:21:56 | 0:22:01 | |
themselves, they have to attach themselves to the female, | 0:22:01 | 0:22:03 | |
and feed off her, like a little vampire. | 0:22:03 | 0:22:06 | |
So, if I hold this very still... | 0:22:06 | 0:22:09 | |
That little, sort of, tadpole thing there, is the male. | 0:22:09 | 0:22:13 | |
And he's actually like a little parasite. | 0:22:13 | 0:22:17 | |
Very judgmental terms here, James. | 0:22:17 | 0:22:19 | |
He's been a vampire, a parasite, and pathetic. | 0:22:19 | 0:22:21 | |
He is a little bit! She's using light, | 0:22:21 | 0:22:23 | |
so she has, like the dragonfish, | 0:22:23 | 0:22:26 | |
she's using light for catching her food. | 0:22:26 | 0:22:28 | |
So that little matchstick-like thing there would be going, | 0:22:28 | 0:22:31 | |
that little blue dot. That's what she uses to lure her prey in. | 0:22:31 | 0:22:34 | |
And this lovely beard that she has, | 0:22:34 | 0:22:36 | |
these have light organs down there, too. | 0:22:36 | 0:22:38 | |
We're not sure what they are used for. | 0:22:38 | 0:22:40 | |
They may even be, sort of, like, landing lights, | 0:22:40 | 0:22:42 | |
to guide the male in. We just don't know. | 0:22:42 | 0:22:44 | |
Thank you very, very much. There's one more thing I needed to ask. | 0:22:44 | 0:22:48 | |
They are making light, how are they actually doing it? | 0:22:48 | 0:22:51 | |
Well, these three are doing it in a very, sort of, chemical manner. | 0:22:51 | 0:22:54 | |
They have two chemicals, I think | 0:22:54 | 0:22:56 | |
we've mentioned them already, luciferin and luciferase. | 0:22:56 | 0:22:58 | |
They combine the two, and that turns the light on, basically. | 0:22:58 | 0:23:01 | |
-Like glow sticks. -Exactly, yes. | 0:23:01 | 0:23:03 | |
But the angler fish is doing it in a much more clever way. | 0:23:03 | 0:23:05 | |
It's actually getting someone else to make the light for it. | 0:23:05 | 0:23:08 | |
She acquires, as she grows, little luminescent bacteria, | 0:23:08 | 0:23:11 | |
and she keeps them, | 0:23:11 | 0:23:12 | |
cultures them, and grows them, | 0:23:12 | 0:23:14 | |
and that's what she uses to make her lights. | 0:23:14 | 0:23:16 | |
Amazing. Thank you very much, James Maclaine, thank you. | 0:23:16 | 0:23:19 | |
I won't shake your hand! | 0:23:19 | 0:23:20 | |
Now, fish are not the only animals that communicate with light. | 0:23:27 | 0:23:30 | |
It's a technique used by jellyfish and squid, | 0:23:30 | 0:23:32 | |
but also above the water by insects like fireflies. | 0:23:32 | 0:23:36 | |
Now, this firefly is using light to attract females. | 0:23:37 | 0:23:41 | |
What is quite interesting about some species of fireflies | 0:23:41 | 0:23:44 | |
is that, in groups, | 0:23:44 | 0:23:46 | |
they start to synchronise their flashes somewhat, | 0:23:46 | 0:23:50 | |
with each other. | 0:23:50 | 0:23:52 | |
Why would they do this? | 0:23:52 | 0:23:53 | |
Why would there be value in flashing your light at the same time as the | 0:23:53 | 0:23:58 | |
other males that you're competing with to attract females? | 0:23:58 | 0:24:02 | |
Well, we're going to have a look at a demonstration. | 0:24:02 | 0:24:05 | |
You're all wearing LED wrist bands. | 0:24:05 | 0:24:07 | |
Can you hold them up next to your head? Fantastic. | 0:24:07 | 0:24:10 | |
And they are going to get turned on. | 0:24:10 | 0:24:13 | |
What we're doing here... | 0:24:14 | 0:24:17 | |
On this screen we've got a camera set up | 0:24:17 | 0:24:19 | |
that's picking up those flashes. | 0:24:19 | 0:24:21 | |
So you can see is something going on in the auditorium | 0:24:21 | 0:24:24 | |
with these random flashes. | 0:24:24 | 0:24:26 | |
If we start to make your wristbands | 0:24:28 | 0:24:31 | |
start to flash more in synchrony with each other, | 0:24:31 | 0:24:34 | |
you start to see something a bit more | 0:24:34 | 0:24:38 | |
coherent appearing on the screen. | 0:24:38 | 0:24:40 | |
It's more salient. It's more noticeable. | 0:24:40 | 0:24:43 | |
When you synchronise your flashes... | 0:24:44 | 0:24:47 | |
..with the other fireflies around you, | 0:24:49 | 0:24:51 | |
what you get is a signal which is much stronger. | 0:24:51 | 0:24:54 | |
And that, potentially, might be more noticeable by the female. | 0:24:54 | 0:24:57 | |
You've still got to compete with those | 0:24:57 | 0:24:59 | |
other males once you've attracted the females, | 0:24:59 | 0:25:01 | |
but maybe you'd be more likely to attract | 0:25:01 | 0:25:03 | |
more females in the first place, if you can be seen. | 0:25:03 | 0:25:06 | |
It's also the case that different species can use different kinds of | 0:25:06 | 0:25:11 | |
flashing sequences to signal what species they are. | 0:25:11 | 0:25:15 | |
Maybe that's also helping the females know | 0:25:15 | 0:25:18 | |
who they are looking for. | 0:25:18 | 0:25:19 | |
Interestingly, in a very similar way, | 0:25:19 | 0:25:21 | |
different UK lighthouses flash lights with different patterns, | 0:25:21 | 0:25:26 | |
so this tells ship captains not only that there is a lighthouse there, | 0:25:26 | 0:25:31 | |
but also which lighthouse it is. | 0:25:31 | 0:25:33 | |
And, interestingly, | 0:25:33 | 0:25:34 | |
that's a technique that was brought to the UK by | 0:25:34 | 0:25:37 | |
Michael Faraday, the man, and the scientist, | 0:25:37 | 0:25:39 | |
who started these lectures here at the Royal Institution. | 0:25:39 | 0:25:42 | |
So we've seen animals that make their own light to communicate, | 0:25:43 | 0:25:47 | |
but daytime land dwellers like us don't need to do that, | 0:25:47 | 0:25:51 | |
because we've got a great big light in the sky. | 0:25:51 | 0:25:53 | |
We've got the sun. | 0:25:53 | 0:25:55 | |
And the sun is making light. | 0:25:55 | 0:25:56 | |
Out in that daylight, | 0:25:56 | 0:25:58 | |
we can be seen. | 0:25:58 | 0:25:59 | |
We can take advantage of the fact that we are visible. | 0:25:59 | 0:26:02 | |
And we can use this to communicate | 0:26:03 | 0:26:04 | |
our state of mind, and our wellbeing. | 0:26:04 | 0:26:06 | |
We can change our body's position, | 0:26:06 | 0:26:08 | |
and how it moves, to convey that. | 0:26:08 | 0:26:10 | |
This is called body language. | 0:26:10 | 0:26:11 | |
And we are not the only animals that use it. | 0:26:11 | 0:26:15 | |
Please welcome Betty and Lola, and their owners, Dani and Martin. | 0:26:15 | 0:26:18 | |
Hello. Hello. | 0:26:22 | 0:26:26 | |
Aren't you lovely? Aren't you lovely? | 0:26:26 | 0:26:28 | |
Now, can I have a volunteer who | 0:26:28 | 0:26:30 | |
might like to come and say hello to a dog? | 0:26:30 | 0:26:32 | |
That's a popular one, that is. | 0:26:32 | 0:26:33 | |
It's going to be very difficult to choose. | 0:26:33 | 0:26:35 | |
I think I'm going to have to ask, can I have you, | 0:26:35 | 0:26:38 | |
on the end? With the white T-shirt, the grey T-shirt. | 0:26:38 | 0:26:41 | |
No, that's it, you're looking behind you. | 0:26:41 | 0:26:43 | |
Thank you very much. Thank you. | 0:26:43 | 0:26:45 | |
Now, very, very gently come and say hello to these good girls. | 0:26:45 | 0:26:50 | |
Hello. There we go. | 0:26:50 | 0:26:53 | |
Now, what does it mean when Betty wags her tail like that? | 0:26:53 | 0:26:57 | |
-AUDIENCE MEMBER: -It means that she's happy. | 0:26:59 | 0:27:01 | |
She's happy, and having a good time, yes, exactly. | 0:27:01 | 0:27:04 | |
We all know this, don't we? A dog wags its tail when it's happy. | 0:27:04 | 0:27:07 | |
Well, the situation turns out to be a lot more nuanced than that. | 0:27:07 | 0:27:11 | |
Scientists from Genoa University | 0:27:12 | 0:27:15 | |
have recently suggested that dogs | 0:27:15 | 0:27:17 | |
will wag their tails slightly differently, | 0:27:17 | 0:27:19 | |
depending on their mood. | 0:27:19 | 0:27:21 | |
What they found is the dog with the | 0:27:21 | 0:27:23 | |
more left-leaning wag is a rather more | 0:27:23 | 0:27:26 | |
calm, relaxed, happy dog. | 0:27:26 | 0:27:28 | |
Whereas a more right-leaning wag | 0:27:28 | 0:27:30 | |
means the dog's a little bit more anxious. | 0:27:30 | 0:27:33 | |
You can see this on the clip here. | 0:27:33 | 0:27:35 | |
That's a more rightwards wag, | 0:27:35 | 0:27:37 | |
slightly more anxious dog. | 0:27:37 | 0:27:38 | |
And here we've got a leftwards wag. | 0:27:38 | 0:27:40 | |
OK. The really interesting part was, | 0:27:40 | 0:27:43 | |
they would show these videos to other dogs. | 0:27:43 | 0:27:48 | |
And the dogs watching these videos | 0:27:48 | 0:27:50 | |
would start to show slightly different emotional responses. | 0:27:50 | 0:27:54 | |
The dogs who are watching the more rightwards leaning wag | 0:27:54 | 0:27:57 | |
showed signs of becoming more anxious. | 0:27:57 | 0:27:59 | |
And the dogs who watched the leftwards leaning wag | 0:27:59 | 0:28:02 | |
became more relaxed. | 0:28:02 | 0:28:04 | |
So dogs really do seem to be sending quite a complex, | 0:28:04 | 0:28:07 | |
quite a nuanced piece of information in addition to just, "I'm happy." | 0:28:07 | 0:28:12 | |
Thank you very much for helping us with the dogs. | 0:28:12 | 0:28:14 | |
Thank you very, very much, Lola, thank you, Betty. | 0:28:14 | 0:28:16 | |
Thank you, Dani. Thank you, Martin. Thank you. | 0:28:16 | 0:28:19 | |
So dogs seem to be really good at reading each other's body language. | 0:28:26 | 0:28:31 | |
But can dogs pick up on body language messages | 0:28:31 | 0:28:34 | |
from another animal? | 0:28:34 | 0:28:35 | |
Like us? Many people will have noticed | 0:28:35 | 0:28:38 | |
their dogs behaving differently, | 0:28:38 | 0:28:40 | |
depending on whether they are in a good or bad mood. | 0:28:40 | 0:28:43 | |
But do those dogs really know what we're thinking? | 0:28:43 | 0:28:46 | |
At the University of Lincoln, | 0:28:46 | 0:28:47 | |
Professor Daniel Mills is testing whether or not | 0:28:47 | 0:28:50 | |
dogs can use our facial expressions to help understand our emotions. | 0:28:50 | 0:28:54 | |
Hiya. So these are some of the team that we've been working with. | 0:28:54 | 0:28:57 | |
These are just pet dogs, | 0:28:57 | 0:28:58 | |
they've not been specially trained for anything to do with the study. | 0:28:58 | 0:29:02 | |
We just want to see how they respond to | 0:29:02 | 0:29:03 | |
what's going on emotionally, around them. | 0:29:03 | 0:29:06 | |
So we want them to have | 0:29:06 | 0:29:07 | |
the full repertoire of emotional reactions, | 0:29:07 | 0:29:09 | |
lunging at cameras, and everything. OK? | 0:29:09 | 0:29:12 | |
What we have is, we have a set-up here, you can see these two screens. | 0:29:17 | 0:29:21 | |
And one of them shows an angry face, one of them shows a happy face. | 0:29:21 | 0:29:25 | |
The dog sits down here, and then we'll play them a sound, | 0:29:25 | 0:29:28 | |
and that will either be an angry or a happy sound. | 0:29:28 | 0:29:31 | |
The language is Portuguese, and Ralph here doesn't know Portuguese. | 0:29:31 | 0:29:35 | |
If they can understand emotion, | 0:29:35 | 0:29:36 | |
then the only thing linking the | 0:29:36 | 0:29:38 | |
sounds and the pictures is their emotional content. | 0:29:38 | 0:29:41 | |
PORTUGUESE SPOKEN WITH HAPPY INTONATION | 0:29:41 | 0:29:43 | |
PORTUGUESE SPOKEN WITH ANGRY INTONATION | 0:29:43 | 0:29:46 | |
What we find is that, actually, | 0:29:46 | 0:29:48 | |
the dogs spend more time looking at the picture that matches the sound. | 0:29:48 | 0:29:52 | |
HAPPY PORTUGUESE | 0:29:52 | 0:29:53 | |
HAPPY PORTUGUESE REPEATED | 0:29:53 | 0:29:56 | |
The only way you can actually do that is | 0:29:56 | 0:29:58 | |
if you can extract the emotional information from both. | 0:29:58 | 0:30:01 | |
ANGRY PORTUGUESE | 0:30:01 | 0:30:03 | |
ANGRY PORTUGUESE REPEATED | 0:30:03 | 0:30:05 | |
Sure enough, the dogs do this | 0:30:05 | 0:30:06 | |
integration of the different emotional signals. | 0:30:06 | 0:30:08 | |
They clearly have this much higher understanding of emotion than | 0:30:08 | 0:30:11 | |
we've previously actually thought. | 0:30:11 | 0:30:13 | |
ANGRY PORTUGUESE | 0:30:13 | 0:30:15 | |
ANGRY PORTUGUESE REPEATED | 0:30:15 | 0:30:17 | |
ANGRY PORTUGUESE REPEATED | 0:30:17 | 0:30:19 | |
So pet dogs really do seem to be | 0:30:25 | 0:30:28 | |
able to detect when there's emotional | 0:30:28 | 0:30:30 | |
information from the face and from the voice, they are coherent, | 0:30:30 | 0:30:34 | |
they are consistent with one another. | 0:30:34 | 0:30:35 | |
So they are definitely using our facial information to work out what | 0:30:35 | 0:30:39 | |
emotion we're expressing. | 0:30:39 | 0:30:41 | |
But how good are we humans at | 0:30:41 | 0:30:43 | |
picking up silent visual communication? | 0:30:43 | 0:30:47 | |
What do you think we are looking at here, | 0:30:50 | 0:30:52 | |
if our pile of ping-pong balls starts to move? | 0:30:52 | 0:30:54 | |
-Do you think we are looking at humans? AUDIENCE: -Yes. | 0:31:03 | 0:31:06 | |
Yeah, OK. How many humans? | 0:31:06 | 0:31:09 | |
-Two. -Do you think it's two men, two women? | 0:31:09 | 0:31:11 | |
-Two men. -A man and a woman? | 0:31:11 | 0:31:15 | |
It's a man and a woman, I think. | 0:31:15 | 0:31:17 | |
OK. Shall we try some emotions? Give us an emotion. | 0:31:17 | 0:31:20 | |
-Scared. -Scared. Perfect. | 0:31:24 | 0:31:25 | |
-OK, try another one. -Angry. | 0:31:25 | 0:31:29 | |
Fantastic. | 0:31:29 | 0:31:30 | |
And one last one. | 0:31:30 | 0:31:31 | |
Happy. | 0:31:32 | 0:31:33 | |
Yes, fantastic. | 0:31:33 | 0:31:35 | |
Laughter, yes. | 0:31:36 | 0:31:37 | |
Fantastic. | 0:31:40 | 0:31:41 | |
Now, this is Blair and Alexis, and in fact, | 0:31:45 | 0:31:47 | |
you were getting all that information | 0:31:47 | 0:31:48 | |
from who they were and how they were moving | 0:31:48 | 0:31:51 | |
just from 14 points which | 0:31:51 | 0:31:52 | |
are marked on their joints for each of them. | 0:31:52 | 0:31:55 | |
So this is showing you how good you are | 0:31:55 | 0:31:57 | |
at pulling out information about how humans move, | 0:31:57 | 0:32:00 | |
because even with this really reduced amount of information, | 0:32:00 | 0:32:03 | |
you could tell a lot about who they were, | 0:32:03 | 0:32:05 | |
what emotions they were feeling. | 0:32:05 | 0:32:06 | |
Thank you, Blair, thank you, Alexis. | 0:32:06 | 0:32:08 | |
Now, our bodies give away a huge amount about our inner feelings, | 0:32:14 | 0:32:18 | |
but our faces can be even more expressive. | 0:32:18 | 0:32:23 | |
I've got a very scary video | 0:32:23 | 0:32:25 | |
that I want you to watch. | 0:32:25 | 0:32:27 | |
Please bear in mind, it is very scary, | 0:32:27 | 0:32:30 | |
but I need you to keep looking, OK? | 0:32:30 | 0:32:33 | |
I want you to watch this video and count how many birds you can see. | 0:32:33 | 0:32:37 | |
I will be testing you. | 0:32:37 | 0:32:38 | |
CALM JINGLY MUSIC | 0:32:42 | 0:32:45 | |
SCREAMING | 0:32:58 | 0:33:00 | |
I've got to tell you, that was a trick. There were no birds. | 0:33:06 | 0:33:09 | |
OK, don't worry if you hadn't seen any birds. | 0:33:09 | 0:33:11 | |
I just wanted you to get scared by the video. | 0:33:11 | 0:33:14 | |
Let's have a look at what we filmed, from you, in the audience, | 0:33:14 | 0:33:17 | |
and see if we've got any good reactions. | 0:33:17 | 0:33:19 | |
Oh, fantastic. | 0:33:24 | 0:33:25 | |
Now, I think that was you. | 0:33:25 | 0:33:28 | |
Did you feel scared? You moved right back, didn't you? | 0:33:28 | 0:33:30 | |
One of the things that's quite interesting, quite striking, | 0:33:31 | 0:33:34 | |
about these sorts of fearful facial emotions, | 0:33:34 | 0:33:38 | |
is that everybody's slightly different. | 0:33:38 | 0:33:40 | |
We're not all doing everything exactly the same way. | 0:33:40 | 0:33:42 | |
But you see these commonalities, for emotions like fear, | 0:33:42 | 0:33:47 | |
if we look back at your disgusted faces | 0:33:47 | 0:33:49 | |
when we were sending that poo-y smell out. | 0:33:49 | 0:33:52 | |
You could see some similarities there. | 0:33:52 | 0:33:54 | |
That's because these are examples of | 0:33:54 | 0:33:56 | |
what are known as universal, or basic emotions. | 0:33:56 | 0:34:00 | |
They mean the same thing wherever you go. | 0:34:00 | 0:34:03 | |
Now, it's not all our emotions, it's things like fear, anger, | 0:34:03 | 0:34:06 | |
and disgust, laughter, | 0:34:06 | 0:34:09 | |
and they are almost like a map of human emotions | 0:34:09 | 0:34:13 | |
that we can use to convey | 0:34:13 | 0:34:15 | |
how we're feeling for a certain set of emotions | 0:34:15 | 0:34:17 | |
that it seems all humans can experience and recognise. | 0:34:17 | 0:34:21 | |
But where do these emotional expressions come from? | 0:34:21 | 0:34:25 | |
Well, the fact that some of them are universal, | 0:34:25 | 0:34:27 | |
you find them across all human cultures, | 0:34:27 | 0:34:29 | |
is a hint that they come from deeper in our evolutionary story. | 0:34:29 | 0:34:34 | |
Hello, Charlotte. Now, this is a book, | 0:34:34 | 0:34:37 | |
a very special book from the RI library. | 0:34:37 | 0:34:40 | |
And it's a book by Charles Darwin. | 0:34:40 | 0:34:41 | |
Now, Charles Darwin is obviously very famous | 0:34:41 | 0:34:44 | |
for The Origin Of Species but | 0:34:44 | 0:34:45 | |
he also wrote this absolutely beautiful book | 0:34:45 | 0:34:48 | |
called On The Expression Of Emotions In Man And Animals. | 0:34:48 | 0:34:51 | |
Now, Charlotte, you are the archivist here | 0:34:51 | 0:34:54 | |
at the Royal Institution, yeah? | 0:34:54 | 0:34:55 | |
Can we have a look at some of the pictures here? | 0:34:55 | 0:34:58 | |
So this is examples that Charles Darwin | 0:34:58 | 0:35:01 | |
was giving of fearful faces. | 0:35:01 | 0:35:03 | |
Has he got a fearful animal in there? | 0:35:03 | 0:35:04 | |
So that's a fearful cat, the cat doesn't look exactly like the human, | 0:35:04 | 0:35:10 | |
the faces are different, but you are getting the open mouth, | 0:35:10 | 0:35:14 | |
he was trying to tease out how this could be working. | 0:35:14 | 0:35:16 | |
Maybe emotions have the same function, | 0:35:16 | 0:35:18 | |
in humans and other animals. | 0:35:18 | 0:35:19 | |
Maybe that's why they are evolutionarily important. | 0:35:19 | 0:35:21 | |
Thank you very much, Charlotte. | 0:35:21 | 0:35:23 | |
And, actually, the work that Darwin started has been | 0:35:27 | 0:35:30 | |
very influential over the next 150 or so years. | 0:35:30 | 0:35:32 | |
Many of his ideas about these motions we share, | 0:35:32 | 0:35:35 | |
across humans and other animals, | 0:35:35 | 0:35:37 | |
that have this ancient evolutionary basis, really have been borne out. | 0:35:37 | 0:35:41 | |
One idea about why they might look the way they do could be simply that | 0:35:42 | 0:35:47 | |
they've evolved to be as distinct as possible. | 0:35:47 | 0:35:49 | |
If I look frightened, you should be | 0:35:49 | 0:35:51 | |
able to recognise that unambiguously, | 0:35:51 | 0:35:53 | |
because if I'm frightened, you should be frightened, too. | 0:35:53 | 0:35:56 | |
So these basic emotions, they can't be uncertain, | 0:35:56 | 0:35:59 | |
they can't be too ambiguous. | 0:35:59 | 0:36:00 | |
You need to be able to pick up on them quickly. | 0:36:00 | 0:36:02 | |
So maybe that's why they look the way they do. | 0:36:02 | 0:36:05 | |
But what about when we don't want to give away our emotions? | 0:36:05 | 0:36:10 | |
A lot of the time, we work quite hard to cover up what we're feeling. | 0:36:10 | 0:36:15 | |
Sometimes because it might be a bit difficult for the social situation, | 0:36:15 | 0:36:19 | |
sometimes because some emotions are considered, in certain cultures, | 0:36:19 | 0:36:22 | |
to be simply inappropriate. | 0:36:22 | 0:36:24 | |
Right now, in our culture, we like to think that boys don't cry. | 0:36:24 | 0:36:27 | |
Boys, you're absolutely allowed to cry, | 0:36:27 | 0:36:29 | |
but we just believe it's not a very manly thing to do. | 0:36:29 | 0:36:31 | |
That's just us. | 0:36:31 | 0:36:33 | |
But it means that people often are | 0:36:33 | 0:36:35 | |
trying to put a face out to the world, | 0:36:35 | 0:36:37 | |
that might cover up the emotions they are really feeling. | 0:36:37 | 0:36:40 | |
And what I'd like to do is do an experiment with you three | 0:36:40 | 0:36:45 | |
to see if we can find out which of you is good at doing this. | 0:36:45 | 0:36:50 | |
Now, to do this what I have are three delicious sweets. | 0:36:50 | 0:36:53 | |
And I would like you each to choose a delicious sweet. | 0:36:53 | 0:36:56 | |
Don't put it in your mouth just yet, take the tissue as well. | 0:36:57 | 0:37:00 | |
Now, before you put them in your mouths, I have to tell you, | 0:37:02 | 0:37:05 | |
one of these delicious sweets is not even slightly delicious. | 0:37:05 | 0:37:08 | |
It's absolutely horrible. What we are going to do - | 0:37:08 | 0:37:11 | |
I don't know which one's got it - | 0:37:11 | 0:37:13 | |
we're going to ask you to put those in your mouths in just a second, | 0:37:13 | 0:37:15 | |
and we're all going to look at you | 0:37:15 | 0:37:18 | |
and see if we can work out which one of you has got the horrible sweet. | 0:37:18 | 0:37:22 | |
So your job is to try and pretend that you're absolutely fine, | 0:37:22 | 0:37:25 | |
you've got a delicious one. OK? We good? | 0:37:25 | 0:37:28 | |
Are we going to count them down? Three, two, | 0:37:28 | 0:37:31 | |
one. | 0:37:31 | 0:37:33 | |
I'm getting something here as well. | 0:37:46 | 0:37:48 | |
Oh. | 0:37:49 | 0:37:51 | |
Now, who thinks this person has the horrible sweet? | 0:37:51 | 0:37:56 | |
Who thinks you've got a horrible sweet? | 0:37:58 | 0:38:01 | |
Who thinks you've got the horrible sweet? | 0:38:02 | 0:38:04 | |
Actually, they've all got horrible sweets, I'm a liar, OK. | 0:38:04 | 0:38:07 | |
They are all horrible sweets. You can spit them out if you want to. | 0:38:07 | 0:38:11 | |
Apparently, if you keep going they become acceptable. | 0:38:11 | 0:38:13 | |
But...you don't have to. | 0:38:13 | 0:38:16 | |
The point here is that even though they were trying very, very hard, | 0:38:16 | 0:38:19 | |
everybody thought they were the only one with a horrible sweet. | 0:38:19 | 0:38:22 | |
But little bits of it were coming through. | 0:38:22 | 0:38:24 | |
We leak through our real feelings all the time, | 0:38:24 | 0:38:27 | |
even if we're trying to control what we look like. | 0:38:27 | 0:38:30 | |
And, of course, that can mean, in reality, | 0:38:30 | 0:38:32 | |
it can be quite complex to work out what someone's face is saying. | 0:38:32 | 0:38:36 | |
There can be a lot of things that they want to express, | 0:38:36 | 0:38:38 | |
and that they are trying to hide, on their face, all at the same time. | 0:38:38 | 0:38:42 | |
What does that mean for people who might encounter challenges when they | 0:38:44 | 0:38:48 | |
are trying to understand other people's emotions? | 0:38:48 | 0:38:51 | |
All this complexity in what faces say | 0:38:52 | 0:38:55 | |
can create real issues for people on the autism spectrum - | 0:38:55 | 0:38:59 | |
real issues recognising what other | 0:38:59 | 0:39:01 | |
people's emotions mean, and, of course, | 0:39:01 | 0:39:03 | |
what their own emotions might be conveying. | 0:39:03 | 0:39:06 | |
I'd like to introduce a robot who is a potential way to help. | 0:39:06 | 0:39:11 | |
Please welcome Zeno the robot | 0:39:11 | 0:39:13 | |
and Dr Alyssa Alcorn from UCL, | 0:39:13 | 0:39:16 | |
part of the DE-ENIGMA project. | 0:39:16 | 0:39:17 | |
Hello, Zeno. | 0:39:25 | 0:39:27 | |
Hey. | 0:39:31 | 0:39:32 | |
Alyssa, how is Zeno going to help? | 0:39:34 | 0:39:37 | |
On the DE-ENIGMA project we're using the Zeno robots as teachers, | 0:39:38 | 0:39:43 | |
as part of a programme working with children on the autism spectrum, | 0:39:43 | 0:39:46 | |
to help them learn about facial expressions | 0:39:46 | 0:39:49 | |
and tell different facial expressions apart, | 0:39:49 | 0:39:52 | |
what they are called, what they mean. | 0:39:52 | 0:39:54 | |
And this will help them in their everyday social interaction. | 0:39:54 | 0:39:58 | |
Fantastic. | 0:39:58 | 0:39:59 | |
Can we find out more about how he works, | 0:39:59 | 0:40:01 | |
or is it easier to do that with a volunteer? | 0:40:01 | 0:40:04 | |
-I think it is, actually. -OK, now, I'd like you... | 0:40:04 | 0:40:08 | |
You can put your hands down. | 0:40:08 | 0:40:09 | |
Just pull your angriest face, | 0:40:09 | 0:40:11 | |
and I'm going to try and find a very good actor. | 0:40:11 | 0:40:14 | |
A really angry face. | 0:40:14 | 0:40:15 | |
Very, very angry faces. | 0:40:16 | 0:40:18 | |
There is a fantastic angry face there, with antlers, can I have you, | 0:40:18 | 0:40:23 | |
angry man? Thank you very much. | 0:40:23 | 0:40:25 | |
Now, just stand there. | 0:40:31 | 0:40:33 | |
-Can you tell me your name? -Addy. | 0:40:33 | 0:40:35 | |
Addy? Fantastic. | 0:40:35 | 0:40:36 | |
Can you just come and sit here? | 0:40:36 | 0:40:38 | |
What we are going to ask you to do | 0:40:38 | 0:40:40 | |
is pull some emotional faces at Zeno. | 0:40:40 | 0:40:44 | |
And we're going to see - look on the screen here - | 0:40:44 | 0:40:46 | |
we'll see how Zeno is seeing that face. Is that right? | 0:40:46 | 0:40:50 | |
So, let's have a look up here. | 0:40:50 | 0:40:52 | |
And we see we've got this green box that appears over his face, | 0:40:52 | 0:40:56 | |
and it's tracking 49 different points on his face, | 0:40:56 | 0:41:00 | |
each little red dot there. | 0:41:00 | 0:41:01 | |
And if you start moving your face for Zeno... | 0:41:01 | 0:41:05 | |
See, the picture is moving, too. | 0:41:05 | 0:41:07 | |
And it's tracking those points, where they are, | 0:41:07 | 0:41:09 | |
how they are moving over time, | 0:41:09 | 0:41:11 | |
and then is using a machine learning process, using statistics, | 0:41:11 | 0:41:15 | |
to estimate what kind of face it thinks he's showing. | 0:41:15 | 0:41:18 | |
So if we look down here, right now, it thinks he's smiling. | 0:41:18 | 0:41:22 | |
I think that looks like a pretty good smile. | 0:41:22 | 0:41:24 | |
-That's a great smile. -Yeah. | 0:41:24 | 0:41:26 | |
Shall we try a different expression? | 0:41:26 | 0:41:27 | |
Could you look frightened for us, | 0:41:27 | 0:41:29 | |
could you pull a screaming face? | 0:41:29 | 0:41:30 | |
So what's happening now is it's taking all those points there, | 0:41:32 | 0:41:36 | |
and using them like instructions for the robot's face. | 0:41:36 | 0:41:39 | |
It's telling him how to move the motors in his face, | 0:41:39 | 0:41:43 | |
so he's making the same expression, at almost the same time. | 0:41:43 | 0:41:46 | |
Fantastic, fantastic. | 0:41:46 | 0:41:48 | |
Can we try the smile again, then? | 0:41:48 | 0:41:49 | |
Turn your face back up. There we go. That's more like it. | 0:41:49 | 0:41:52 | |
Zeno is smiling, too. | 0:41:52 | 0:41:54 | |
You have an officially intense smile, that's quite impressive. | 0:41:54 | 0:41:56 | |
Thank you very much. | 0:41:56 | 0:41:58 | |
-Thank you, Alyssa. -Thank you. | 0:41:58 | 0:42:00 | |
Thank you, Addy. Thank you. | 0:42:03 | 0:42:05 | |
It's really exciting to see how these modern-day robotics could be | 0:42:09 | 0:42:13 | |
possibly used to really help people who are struggling to understand the | 0:42:13 | 0:42:17 | |
nuances of facial expressions. | 0:42:17 | 0:42:19 | |
We've seen how amazingly complex facial expressions | 0:42:19 | 0:42:23 | |
can be on their own in humans, | 0:42:23 | 0:42:24 | |
but there's another part of our face | 0:42:24 | 0:42:26 | |
that communicates even more - our eyes. | 0:42:26 | 0:42:29 | |
In contrast to other mammals, humans have got very, very different eyes. | 0:42:29 | 0:42:35 | |
Our eyes are the same structure, | 0:42:35 | 0:42:37 | |
we've got the whites of the eyes, a pupil, and the iris. | 0:42:37 | 0:42:40 | |
But in all other mammals you can only see the iris and the pupil. | 0:42:40 | 0:42:44 | |
In human eyes you can absolutely | 0:42:44 | 0:42:45 | |
always see the whites of the eyes around that. | 0:42:45 | 0:42:47 | |
You are seeing the sclera. | 0:42:47 | 0:42:49 | |
And that seems to do two things. | 0:42:49 | 0:42:51 | |
It makes the eyes really stand out, our eyes are very high contrast, | 0:42:51 | 0:42:56 | |
very visible things. | 0:42:56 | 0:42:57 | |
And also, it lets us be very, very sensitive | 0:42:57 | 0:43:01 | |
to where exactly someone is looking. | 0:43:01 | 0:43:04 | |
And we use that all the time when we're having communication, | 0:43:04 | 0:43:08 | |
when we're talking to each other. | 0:43:08 | 0:43:09 | |
So if I just do a quick demonstration here, | 0:43:09 | 0:43:11 | |
I'm going to look down this camera. | 0:43:11 | 0:43:13 | |
And I'm going to tell you when I move my eyes, | 0:43:13 | 0:43:15 | |
and I want you to tell me if I'm looking directly at you, | 0:43:15 | 0:43:18 | |
or if I'm looking away. | 0:43:18 | 0:43:20 | |
OK. I'll start with my eyes closed, | 0:43:20 | 0:43:22 | |
and I'll try and move them every time I open my eyes. | 0:43:22 | 0:43:24 | |
OK. | 0:43:24 | 0:43:25 | |
At you or away? | 0:43:27 | 0:43:28 | |
At me. | 0:43:30 | 0:43:31 | |
Away. | 0:43:32 | 0:43:34 | |
Now, you're picking really, really tiny movements of my eyes up there. | 0:43:34 | 0:43:39 | |
I was actually moving my eyes only just to the side of the camera. | 0:43:39 | 0:43:43 | |
About two degrees of the whole visual scene around me. | 0:43:43 | 0:43:46 | |
So a minuscule amount. | 0:43:46 | 0:43:48 | |
We are incredibly sensitive to this, and what we do with that | 0:43:48 | 0:43:51 | |
is we use a lot of information about who someone is talking to, | 0:43:51 | 0:43:55 | |
what they are talking about, | 0:43:55 | 0:43:57 | |
and who they are talking about. | 0:43:57 | 0:43:59 | |
And we use it all the time. | 0:43:59 | 0:44:02 | |
Now, thanks for this, | 0:44:02 | 0:44:03 | |
I just want to ask you, do you think, | 0:44:03 | 0:44:05 | |
if I throw this sweet in the air... | 0:44:05 | 0:44:07 | |
..I'll be able to catch it? | 0:44:07 | 0:44:09 | |
I'm very good at this. OK. | 0:44:09 | 0:44:11 | |
Let's try this. | 0:44:11 | 0:44:13 | |
That wasn't very satisfactory, was it? Let's try again. | 0:44:16 | 0:44:18 | |
OK. That's better. Let's try a higher one. | 0:44:21 | 0:44:24 | |
OK. Now, can you throw this to me? | 0:44:26 | 0:44:28 | |
Oh, thank you. Now, can you throw it to me? | 0:44:31 | 0:44:34 | |
Mate, stop it, thank you. | 0:44:36 | 0:44:38 | |
Now, obviously, there were no sweets there whatsoever. | 0:44:38 | 0:44:42 | |
It was just a trick. | 0:44:42 | 0:44:43 | |
What I was doing was looking at where there was nothing, | 0:44:43 | 0:44:46 | |
and I was pointing at where there was nothing, | 0:44:46 | 0:44:48 | |
and I was pretending to hold nothing, | 0:44:48 | 0:44:50 | |
and we all just start looking at nothing | 0:44:50 | 0:44:52 | |
and pretending that I am throwing something in the air | 0:44:52 | 0:44:55 | |
and catching it in my bag. This is the same thing that we're using | 0:44:55 | 0:44:58 | |
when we use eye gaze in conversation | 0:44:58 | 0:45:01 | |
for kind of capturing attention. | 0:45:01 | 0:45:03 | |
We are focusing attention on who we are talking to, | 0:45:03 | 0:45:05 | |
who we are talking about. | 0:45:05 | 0:45:06 | |
For a long time, it was thought this kind of joint attention | 0:45:07 | 0:45:10 | |
was something that only humans could do. | 0:45:10 | 0:45:13 | |
It's turned out other animals can do this, too. | 0:45:13 | 0:45:16 | |
Dogs understand what pointing means. | 0:45:16 | 0:45:19 | |
And even, it turns out, some birds | 0:45:19 | 0:45:22 | |
can understand what eye gaze means. | 0:45:22 | 0:45:24 | |
Ravens are one of the few animals that seem to understand this. | 0:45:24 | 0:45:29 | |
They understand what it means when something is looking. | 0:45:29 | 0:45:31 | |
And I'd like you to give a not too loud clap | 0:45:32 | 0:45:35 | |
to a very fantastic raven called Bran, | 0:45:35 | 0:45:37 | |
and his owner, Lloyd Buck. | 0:45:37 | 0:45:39 | |
Hello. Hello, Lloyd. | 0:45:42 | 0:45:44 | |
Hello. | 0:45:44 | 0:45:46 | |
Hello, Bran. Now, Lloyd, | 0:45:46 | 0:45:48 | |
do you get the feeling that Bran uses information about where you're | 0:45:48 | 0:45:51 | |
-looking? -Yes, very much so. -Can we see an example of that, Lloyd? | 0:45:51 | 0:45:54 | |
We certainly can. Now, Bran has a special stone, he knows that word. | 0:45:54 | 0:45:59 | |
And it's hidden somewhere in this room. | 0:45:59 | 0:46:00 | |
So if I let him eat that piece of food, | 0:46:00 | 0:46:02 | |
then I'll say, where's your stone, Bran? Where is it? | 0:46:02 | 0:46:04 | |
And he's looking, and then if I look, where's your stone, Bran? | 0:46:04 | 0:46:08 | |
He did. He was really looking. | 0:46:13 | 0:46:15 | |
He looked around, and then looked where you were looking. | 0:46:15 | 0:46:18 | |
There's your stone. | 0:46:18 | 0:46:20 | |
Now, Bran. That's a good boy. Thank you very much. | 0:46:20 | 0:46:24 | |
Luckily, Bran chose the person in | 0:46:24 | 0:46:27 | |
the room we know is most easily scared. | 0:46:27 | 0:46:29 | |
Are you going to be OK? | 0:46:29 | 0:46:31 | |
That's amazing. But why would ravens be particularly good at this ability | 0:46:33 | 0:46:37 | |
to understand what it means when something is looking somewhere? | 0:46:37 | 0:46:41 | |
Well, it could be because it's very | 0:46:41 | 0:46:43 | |
useful as a way of helping them solve problems. | 0:46:43 | 0:46:45 | |
These are birds that have | 0:46:45 | 0:46:46 | |
to solve a lot of problems in the wild, aren't they? | 0:46:46 | 0:46:49 | |
Before we actually go into an example of this, | 0:46:49 | 0:46:51 | |
we're just going to show you how good Bran is at solving problems. | 0:46:51 | 0:46:55 | |
And we've got here a puzzle box Bran hasn't seen before. | 0:46:55 | 0:47:00 | |
It's got some food in it that Bran is going to want. | 0:47:00 | 0:47:03 | |
And what we are going to do is show it to him, | 0:47:03 | 0:47:05 | |
and he's going to work out how to | 0:47:05 | 0:47:07 | |
solve the problem of getting through to the stuff he wants in the middle. | 0:47:07 | 0:47:11 | |
-Is that OK? Are we all right to begin? -Yeah, we certainly are. | 0:47:11 | 0:47:13 | |
Go on, then, Bran. | 0:47:15 | 0:47:16 | |
He's got to work out how to get into that... | 0:47:18 | 0:47:20 | |
Unbelievable. | 0:47:33 | 0:47:35 | |
That's it. | 0:47:44 | 0:47:46 | |
I've made it hard for him, this is not an easy one. | 0:47:46 | 0:47:49 | |
He just gave us a right look, like, "Oh, come on!" | 0:47:50 | 0:47:53 | |
Wow. | 0:47:58 | 0:47:59 | |
So he's got to pull the string. | 0:48:03 | 0:48:05 | |
-Yeah. -This is an emotional roller-coaster. | 0:48:05 | 0:48:08 | |
-He'll get it. -There we go, fantastic, well done. | 0:48:08 | 0:48:11 | |
Well done, Bran. | 0:48:14 | 0:48:15 | |
Now, in the wild, | 0:48:17 | 0:48:18 | |
a bird like Bran has got to solve puzzles and problems all the time. | 0:48:18 | 0:48:22 | |
And one of them is there are lots of birds like Bran out there | 0:48:22 | 0:48:24 | |
who are also very clever, so when Bran wants to hide his food, | 0:48:24 | 0:48:28 | |
he has got to find some way of tricking other birds | 0:48:28 | 0:48:32 | |
if they are looking at him. | 0:48:32 | 0:48:33 | |
Now, I've got a clip of a bird doing some deception, | 0:48:33 | 0:48:39 | |
a bird using communication to lie to other birds. | 0:48:39 | 0:48:43 | |
But Bran really dislikes videos. | 0:48:43 | 0:48:45 | |
So we're going to say thank you to Lloyd, | 0:48:45 | 0:48:47 | |
and thank you to the extremely clever Bran before we show that. | 0:48:47 | 0:48:49 | |
Thank you. | 0:48:49 | 0:48:51 | |
This is a bird that is being watched by other birds, | 0:48:55 | 0:48:57 | |
who all saw her bury her food earlier. | 0:48:57 | 0:48:59 | |
Now she knows they are no longer watching, | 0:49:00 | 0:49:02 | |
she's hiding the food elsewhere. | 0:49:02 | 0:49:04 | |
This is typical of raven and crow behaviour. | 0:49:04 | 0:49:07 | |
So, this is an example of ravens | 0:49:07 | 0:49:09 | |
using their understanding of eye gaze to | 0:49:09 | 0:49:11 | |
help them solve the problem, | 0:49:11 | 0:49:13 | |
of how they stop other ravens from stealing all their food. | 0:49:13 | 0:49:15 | |
And it's an interesting point, | 0:49:16 | 0:49:17 | |
that what you're seeing is them using deception | 0:49:17 | 0:49:20 | |
as a way of covering up what they really want to do. | 0:49:20 | 0:49:23 | |
But, of course, deception is still a form of communication. | 0:49:23 | 0:49:27 | |
So we've seen how much we can communicate | 0:49:27 | 0:49:30 | |
with our bodies and our faces, | 0:49:30 | 0:49:32 | |
even if we choose not to, or were trying to fake it. | 0:49:32 | 0:49:35 | |
There is a very basic communication | 0:49:35 | 0:49:37 | |
we do all the time that we're normally a lot less aware of. | 0:49:37 | 0:49:40 | |
And this is how we communicate a lot of friendship and affection, | 0:49:40 | 0:49:44 | |
and affiliation with other people, | 0:49:44 | 0:49:46 | |
by picking up little bits of behaviour, | 0:49:46 | 0:49:48 | |
falling into the same things that they are doing. | 0:49:48 | 0:49:51 | |
A lot of the time, we often do this | 0:49:51 | 0:49:53 | |
without even noticing we're doing it. | 0:49:53 | 0:49:56 | |
A few days ago we filmed two of you going for a walk together. | 0:49:56 | 0:49:59 | |
Are you here? We filmed you walking. | 0:49:59 | 0:50:02 | |
We've got a short clip of this here. | 0:50:02 | 0:50:03 | |
Notice what they are already doing. | 0:50:05 | 0:50:07 | |
They are walking in step with each other. | 0:50:08 | 0:50:10 | |
When you walk alongside somebody else, | 0:50:11 | 0:50:13 | |
you more often than not walk at the same rate as they walk, | 0:50:13 | 0:50:17 | |
even if they are a very different height from you. | 0:50:17 | 0:50:20 | |
And that's an example of this kind of | 0:50:20 | 0:50:22 | |
picking up other people's behaviour. | 0:50:22 | 0:50:24 | |
Just falling in with what they are doing, | 0:50:24 | 0:50:25 | |
without even realising that you're doing it. | 0:50:25 | 0:50:27 | |
You're doing a basic, friendly thing. | 0:50:27 | 0:50:30 | |
Now, you just very politely clapped along with me then. | 0:50:38 | 0:50:41 | |
I had no reason to clap, their walking was good, | 0:50:41 | 0:50:44 | |
it wasn't quite clap, was it? Now, what you are doing is, | 0:50:44 | 0:50:46 | |
you started clapping because I started clapping. | 0:50:46 | 0:50:49 | |
Another example of this, joining in, this, kind of, contagious behaviour. | 0:50:49 | 0:50:53 | |
Studies have shown people in | 0:50:53 | 0:50:54 | |
audiences are much more likely to clap if | 0:50:54 | 0:50:57 | |
somebody near them starts clapping, than if they don't. | 0:50:57 | 0:50:59 | |
It just spreads through the audience, like we just did then. | 0:50:59 | 0:51:02 | |
And, in fact, this kind of contagious behaviour, | 0:51:02 | 0:51:04 | |
where you join in with what other people are doing, | 0:51:04 | 0:51:07 | |
is extremely common. | 0:51:07 | 0:51:08 | |
Laughter, which I study, for example, is highly contagious. | 0:51:08 | 0:51:12 | |
In fact, you are primed to laugh, | 0:51:12 | 0:51:14 | |
just by other people being around you. | 0:51:14 | 0:51:16 | |
You're 13 times more likely to laugh | 0:51:16 | 0:51:18 | |
if there is somebody else with you, | 0:51:18 | 0:51:20 | |
than if you are on your own. | 0:51:20 | 0:51:22 | |
And these contagious behaviours | 0:51:22 | 0:51:24 | |
are still a very important form of communication. | 0:51:24 | 0:51:28 | |
Why does it happen? | 0:51:29 | 0:51:30 | |
What triggers it? Well, this is a picture from a recent study... | 0:51:30 | 0:51:34 | |
..that we did at UCL. | 0:51:36 | 0:51:37 | |
This is showing the brain networks activated | 0:51:37 | 0:51:40 | |
in teenage boys when they listened to laughter. | 0:51:40 | 0:51:44 | |
What we are seeing is a couple of different networks | 0:51:44 | 0:51:47 | |
actually being driven by listening to laughter. | 0:51:47 | 0:51:50 | |
These areas here... | 0:51:50 | 0:51:51 | |
..those are auditory parts of the brain. | 0:51:52 | 0:51:55 | |
And what they are doing is they are | 0:51:55 | 0:51:56 | |
processing the sound of the laughter. | 0:51:56 | 0:51:58 | |
These areas here... | 0:51:58 | 0:51:59 | |
..up there, and down there, | 0:52:01 | 0:52:02 | |
those are brain areas associated with producing movements. | 0:52:02 | 0:52:05 | |
What we are actually seeing here is when you listen to laughter, | 0:52:05 | 0:52:08 | |
even if you're having your brain scanned, | 0:52:08 | 0:52:10 | |
which is not remotely funny, | 0:52:10 | 0:52:12 | |
you are getting ready to join in. | 0:52:12 | 0:52:14 | |
Why on Earth would it be a useful form of communication | 0:52:14 | 0:52:18 | |
to just join in with what somebody else is doing? | 0:52:18 | 0:52:21 | |
Well, it's probably because we are social animals. | 0:52:22 | 0:52:24 | |
We rely on other members of our group for survival. | 0:52:24 | 0:52:27 | |
Picking up little bits of other | 0:52:27 | 0:52:29 | |
people's behaviour and joining in with them, | 0:52:29 | 0:52:31 | |
or reflecting back to them, | 0:52:31 | 0:52:33 | |
is a really quick and efficient communication that you know someone. | 0:52:33 | 0:52:37 | |
You like someone. | 0:52:37 | 0:52:39 | |
And this form of communication, by joint action, may, in fact, | 0:52:39 | 0:52:43 | |
be one of the most important things we ever do. | 0:52:43 | 0:52:46 | |
So we've seen how we can get across our thoughts, feelings, | 0:52:46 | 0:52:50 | |
our intentions, without necessarily ever uttering a word. | 0:52:50 | 0:52:54 | |
But all these forms of communication involve us using our senses to pick | 0:52:54 | 0:52:58 | |
up the messages - we are smelling things, we are seeing things - | 0:52:58 | 0:53:02 | |
could there be a possible future of silent communication | 0:53:02 | 0:53:06 | |
where we just communicate directly, brain to brain? | 0:53:06 | 0:53:09 | |
Well, 40 years ago, someone at NASA definitely thought so. | 0:53:10 | 0:53:14 | |
It's 40 years now since the two Voyager space probes left the Earth. | 0:53:15 | 0:53:19 | |
They each carry a Golden Record. | 0:53:19 | 0:53:22 | |
A bit like this. And there's a sound on the Golden Records, | 0:53:22 | 0:53:26 | |
that not many people know about. | 0:53:26 | 0:53:27 | |
LOW GRINDING HUM | 0:53:27 | 0:53:35 | |
Now, those are electrical signals from someone's brain. | 0:53:35 | 0:53:39 | |
And they've been converted into a sound that we can hear. | 0:53:39 | 0:53:42 | |
The idea behind including these on the record was that an alien species | 0:53:42 | 0:53:47 | |
might be able to read the thoughts that lie behind those brainwaves. | 0:53:47 | 0:53:52 | |
Now, reading brainwaves sounds like science fiction. | 0:53:52 | 0:53:55 | |
How far off the mark were they 40 years ago? | 0:53:55 | 0:53:58 | |
Well, Dr Ioannis Zoulias | 0:53:58 | 0:54:00 | |
and his colleagues from the University of Reading | 0:54:00 | 0:54:03 | |
have devised an experiment to see | 0:54:03 | 0:54:05 | |
if we can prove whether or not this is at all possible. | 0:54:05 | 0:54:08 | |
We've pre-selected a volunteer, Hannah, | 0:54:08 | 0:54:11 | |
to be the person who's going to send us a message from her brain. | 0:54:11 | 0:54:15 | |
She's sitting at the other end of the RI building. | 0:54:15 | 0:54:17 | |
She's wearing a headset, | 0:54:18 | 0:54:20 | |
and that headset is measuring, detecting and measuring, | 0:54:20 | 0:54:24 | |
the electrical activity inside her brain. | 0:54:24 | 0:54:27 | |
On the screen in front of her are two squares. | 0:54:27 | 0:54:31 | |
One is yellow, one's red. | 0:54:31 | 0:54:33 | |
And we're going to tell Hannah which one to look at, | 0:54:33 | 0:54:36 | |
and we're going to read from her brainwaves, | 0:54:36 | 0:54:38 | |
and try and decode that information. | 0:54:38 | 0:54:41 | |
Hannah's mother, Rashima, is going to receive this message. | 0:54:43 | 0:54:48 | |
Now, Rashima, you've got electrodes on your arms, haven't you? | 0:54:48 | 0:54:51 | |
So that suggests you might be | 0:54:51 | 0:54:53 | |
receiving that information via painful electrical shock. | 0:54:53 | 0:54:56 | |
And we're also going to need you to wear this blindfold, | 0:54:56 | 0:55:00 | |
so we definitely know that you can | 0:55:00 | 0:55:02 | |
only be getting the information through the signals to your arms. | 0:55:02 | 0:55:06 | |
And you're going to press one of these two buttons, | 0:55:06 | 0:55:08 | |
depending on if it's your left | 0:55:08 | 0:55:09 | |
or your right arm that is getting the signal. | 0:55:09 | 0:55:13 | |
-Sure. -OK. | 0:55:13 | 0:55:15 | |
We've been testing this all day, | 0:55:17 | 0:55:19 | |
we've got nine results now, | 0:55:19 | 0:55:21 | |
and we're going to run the tenth | 0:55:21 | 0:55:22 | |
and final trial here. | 0:55:22 | 0:55:25 | |
For this last trial I need a | 0:55:25 | 0:55:27 | |
volunteer to select a message that we're going to send to Hannah. | 0:55:27 | 0:55:30 | |
Yellow or red? | 0:55:30 | 0:55:32 | |
OK. Can I ask you? | 0:55:32 | 0:55:34 | |
Just pick one of those. | 0:55:35 | 0:55:36 | |
Don't open it. This is the one you picked, get rid of that one. | 0:55:38 | 0:55:42 | |
Thank you. | 0:55:43 | 0:55:46 | |
Now, I'm going to give this to Natalie, | 0:55:46 | 0:55:48 | |
she's going to run this down to Hannah, | 0:55:48 | 0:55:49 | |
and she's going to show it to Hannah, | 0:55:49 | 0:55:52 | |
and we don't know what this says. | 0:55:52 | 0:55:54 | |
So what the computer is going to do is try and decode the activity in | 0:55:54 | 0:55:57 | |
Hannah's brain, and send this to Rashima's arms. | 0:55:57 | 0:56:00 | |
OK. Natalie, | 0:56:00 | 0:56:02 | |
can I just give you this? Have a very quick look. | 0:56:02 | 0:56:05 | |
Thank you very much. | 0:56:05 | 0:56:07 | |
When Hannah gets the paper she'll look at the chosen colour, | 0:56:07 | 0:56:11 | |
and the computer will interpret her brainwaves | 0:56:11 | 0:56:13 | |
and stimulate either Rashima's right or left arm. | 0:56:13 | 0:56:17 | |
There is Hannah. | 0:56:17 | 0:56:19 | |
OK, so Hannah is going to look at the screen, | 0:56:21 | 0:56:23 | |
she's looking at one of those two squares. | 0:56:23 | 0:56:26 | |
Which button is it going to be? | 0:56:33 | 0:56:36 | |
Red. Amazing, it's right. | 0:56:36 | 0:56:39 | |
Fantastic. | 0:56:39 | 0:56:41 | |
That makes ten out of ten successful trials we've run today. | 0:56:45 | 0:56:49 | |
And I think that definitely deserves a very big finish. | 0:56:49 | 0:56:53 | |
So what we're going to do is blow some stuff up. | 0:56:53 | 0:56:56 | |
This is going to be some noise. | 0:56:56 | 0:56:59 | |
Count me down, three, two, one... | 0:56:59 | 0:57:02 | |
I love the smell of burning balloons. | 0:57:13 | 0:57:17 | |
It smells like science! | 0:57:17 | 0:57:19 | |
Now, this is just a teeny, | 0:57:19 | 0:57:21 | |
tiny step towards the idea of being | 0:57:21 | 0:57:23 | |
able to send information directly from brain to brain. | 0:57:23 | 0:57:26 | |
We are nowhere near being able to communicate complex | 0:57:26 | 0:57:29 | |
thoughts via an electrical signal. | 0:57:29 | 0:57:31 | |
The average adult, for example, knows 35,000 words. | 0:57:31 | 0:57:34 | |
You would need a lot of electrodes | 0:57:34 | 0:57:36 | |
to be able to know which one of those that person was thinking of. | 0:57:36 | 0:57:40 | |
But every science has to start somewhere. | 0:57:40 | 0:57:44 | |
And maybe NASA weren't that far off | 0:57:44 | 0:57:47 | |
the mark with their recording for the aliens. | 0:57:47 | 0:57:51 | |
What I hope I've shown in this lecture is that we | 0:57:51 | 0:57:53 | |
and other animals already | 0:57:53 | 0:57:54 | |
communicate vast amounts of information in a | 0:57:54 | 0:57:57 | |
huge variety of ways, | 0:57:57 | 0:57:58 | |
without ever opening our mouths. | 0:57:58 | 0:58:01 | |
Now, we humans, we might not be using smells to communicate, | 0:58:01 | 0:58:05 | |
but we've replaced that with | 0:58:05 | 0:58:06 | |
an incredible ability to exchange messages with our bodies, | 0:58:06 | 0:58:10 | |
our faces, and our eyes. | 0:58:10 | 0:58:13 | |
If you can't pick up on this world of silent signals and hidden codes, | 0:58:13 | 0:58:18 | |
then you are definitely not getting the full story. | 0:58:18 | 0:58:23 | |
Thank you. | 0:58:23 | 0:58:24 |