Browse content similar to Finding the Way. Check below for episodes and series from the same categories and more!
Line | From | To | |
---|---|---|---|
The natural world is full of extraordinary animals | 0:00:02 | 0:00:06 | |
with amazing life histories. | 0:00:06 | 0:00:08 | |
Yet certain stories are more intriguing than others. | 0:00:08 | 0:00:12 | |
The mysteries of a butterfly's life cycle. | 0:00:14 | 0:00:18 | |
Or the strange biology of the Emperor Penguin. | 0:00:18 | 0:00:20 | |
Some of these creatures were surrounded by | 0:00:22 | 0:00:25 | |
fantastic myths and misunderstandings. | 0:00:25 | 0:00:28 | |
Others have only recently revealed their secrets. | 0:00:29 | 0:00:32 | |
These are the creatures that stand out from the crowd, | 0:00:34 | 0:00:38 | |
the curiosities that I find particularly fascinating. | 0:00:38 | 0:00:42 | |
Pigeons are so common that we tend to take them for granted. | 0:00:49 | 0:00:53 | |
But in fact, they are superb navigators | 0:00:53 | 0:00:56 | |
and can find their way home from hundreds of miles away. | 0:00:56 | 0:01:00 | |
And the dung beetle will roll a dung ball in a straight line | 0:01:01 | 0:01:05 | |
even though it's head down and walking backwards. | 0:01:05 | 0:01:08 | |
How on earth do these creatures manage to find their way? | 0:01:09 | 0:01:13 | |
Pigeon fanciers know that if they take homing pigeons | 0:01:21 | 0:01:25 | |
to somewhere where they've never been before, | 0:01:25 | 0:01:27 | |
even if it's tens, even hundreds of miles away, and then release them, | 0:01:27 | 0:01:32 | |
they will, after circling in the sky, head for home. | 0:01:32 | 0:01:37 | |
These come from over there. | 0:01:38 | 0:01:41 | |
Let's see what happens. | 0:01:41 | 0:01:43 | |
Well, they certainly seem to be heading in the right direction. | 0:02:08 | 0:02:12 | |
But how do pigeons find their way back home | 0:02:12 | 0:02:15 | |
over totally unfamiliar territory? | 0:02:15 | 0:02:18 | |
It seems the more mysterious to us because it's something we can't do | 0:02:18 | 0:02:22 | |
unless we have all kinds of special equipment. | 0:02:22 | 0:02:26 | |
But science is now beginning to find some of the answers. | 0:02:26 | 0:02:29 | |
The homing pigeon is a domesticated descendant of the wild rock pigeon | 0:02:31 | 0:02:36 | |
which lives on cliffs and ledges along our coasts. | 0:02:36 | 0:02:40 | |
When humans started to domesticate them, they inevitably selected | 0:02:40 | 0:02:44 | |
those birds that were particularly good at returning to their roosts. | 0:02:44 | 0:02:49 | |
Pigeons are thought to be the very first birds | 0:02:52 | 0:02:56 | |
to be domesticated by mankind, many thousands of years ago. | 0:02:56 | 0:03:00 | |
Since then, they've been bred into many different varieties, | 0:03:00 | 0:03:04 | |
including homing pigeons. | 0:03:04 | 0:03:06 | |
And the varied appearance of this flock around me | 0:03:06 | 0:03:09 | |
is evidence of that domesticated past. | 0:03:09 | 0:03:13 | |
Since then, of course, they've come to live alongside us in our cities | 0:03:13 | 0:03:18 | |
and giving many of us a lot of pleasure. | 0:03:18 | 0:03:21 | |
Come on. | 0:03:21 | 0:03:22 | |
It was not only their homing ability that made pigeons | 0:03:26 | 0:03:29 | |
so popular with breeders, they are also superb flyers. | 0:03:29 | 0:03:34 | |
Their powerful wing muscles make up nearly half their body weight | 0:03:35 | 0:03:40 | |
and enable them to fly at speeds of 60mph. | 0:03:40 | 0:03:43 | |
They're among the greatest long-distance athletes | 0:03:44 | 0:03:47 | |
in the bird world. | 0:03:47 | 0:03:49 | |
And pigeon fanciers have delighted in breeding them | 0:03:54 | 0:03:58 | |
in order to race them competitively. | 0:03:58 | 0:04:00 | |
Mankind has also used pigeons for a very practical purpose... | 0:04:08 | 0:04:13 | |
..as messengers. | 0:04:15 | 0:04:16 | |
It was during the two World Wars that pigeons played a crucial role | 0:04:19 | 0:04:24 | |
in carrying messages home from the front line. | 0:04:24 | 0:04:27 | |
Nearly a quarter of a million birds served in the wars | 0:04:28 | 0:04:31 | |
and helped save thousands of lives. | 0:04:31 | 0:04:34 | |
One such bird was Cher Ami, | 0:04:34 | 0:04:37 | |
a female donated by a British pigeon fancier | 0:04:37 | 0:04:41 | |
for use by the United States Army during World War I. | 0:04:41 | 0:04:45 | |
During one battle in France in 1918, more than 500 soldiers | 0:04:47 | 0:04:52 | |
from the US infantry became trapped behind enemy lines. | 0:04:52 | 0:04:55 | |
Worse, they were then fired upon by their own troops | 0:04:57 | 0:05:00 | |
who didn't know they were there. | 0:05:00 | 0:05:02 | |
Within 24 hours, more than half of them had been killed. | 0:05:03 | 0:05:07 | |
With no other options, the commander, Major Whittlesey, | 0:05:10 | 0:05:13 | |
desperately tried to send messages back by pigeon. | 0:05:13 | 0:05:17 | |
The first two birds were shot down and only Cher Ami was left. | 0:05:17 | 0:05:21 | |
For several moments, she flew with bullets zipping all around her, | 0:05:22 | 0:05:26 | |
and eventually she was brought down. | 0:05:26 | 0:05:28 | |
She'd been shot through the breast, blinded in one eye | 0:05:28 | 0:05:32 | |
and her leg was left hanging by a tendon. | 0:05:32 | 0:05:35 | |
Astonishingly, the injured bird managed to take flight again | 0:05:36 | 0:05:40 | |
and arrived back at her loft at the division headquarters | 0:05:40 | 0:05:43 | |
in just 25 minutes. | 0:05:43 | 0:05:46 | |
Cher Ami's message helped save the lives of 194 soldiers. | 0:05:47 | 0:05:52 | |
Her name in French, of course, means Dear Friend. | 0:05:53 | 0:05:58 | |
When the Second World War broke out, | 0:06:01 | 0:06:04 | |
Britain's pigeon fanciers once again | 0:06:04 | 0:06:06 | |
gave their pigeons to the war effort | 0:06:06 | 0:06:09 | |
to provide what was called the National Pigeon Service. | 0:06:09 | 0:06:13 | |
This elite squad of birds was extremely valuable and successful. | 0:06:13 | 0:06:18 | |
98% of their messages got through. | 0:06:18 | 0:06:21 | |
The Germans, realising how important the pigeons had become, | 0:06:24 | 0:06:28 | |
fought back with specially trained pigeon snipers. | 0:06:28 | 0:06:31 | |
They also retaliated in another way, | 0:06:37 | 0:06:40 | |
by using the pigeon's natural enemies - peregrine falcons. | 0:06:40 | 0:06:44 | |
Pigeons played such a vital role in the war | 0:06:50 | 0:06:52 | |
that many were celebrated as heroes. | 0:06:52 | 0:06:55 | |
In 1943, this medal, the Dickin Medal, | 0:06:55 | 0:07:00 | |
was founded to honour these animals. | 0:07:00 | 0:07:03 | |
It was awarded to individuals that appeared, | 0:07:03 | 0:07:06 | |
if you'll allow a little anthropomorphism, | 0:07:06 | 0:07:08 | |
to have displayed particular bravery. | 0:07:08 | 0:07:11 | |
And it's sometimes referred to as the Victoria Cross for animals. | 0:07:11 | 0:07:16 | |
Of the 54 medals presented during World War II, | 0:07:16 | 0:07:21 | |
32 were given to pigeons. | 0:07:21 | 0:07:23 | |
We may have prized pigeons for centuries | 0:07:25 | 0:07:28 | |
for their skills in finding their way through the skies, | 0:07:28 | 0:07:31 | |
but for a long time we had no idea how the birds did it. | 0:07:31 | 0:07:35 | |
Some suggested that pigeons used the sun. | 0:07:38 | 0:07:40 | |
Others that they were guided by the Earth's magnetic field. | 0:07:40 | 0:07:44 | |
But proving either was surprisingly difficult. | 0:07:46 | 0:07:49 | |
A breakthrough came from an American scientist | 0:07:53 | 0:07:55 | |
at Cornell University in the 1960s. | 0:07:55 | 0:07:58 | |
William Keeton attached magnets to the backs of some pigeons, | 0:08:01 | 0:08:04 | |
but not to others, | 0:08:04 | 0:08:06 | |
and found that when the sun was out, | 0:08:06 | 0:08:08 | |
both groups were able to make their way home. | 0:08:08 | 0:08:12 | |
But when the sun was hidden behind clouds, | 0:08:13 | 0:08:16 | |
the birds carrying magnets got lost. | 0:08:16 | 0:08:18 | |
Keeton deduced that this was because the signals from their magnets | 0:08:20 | 0:08:24 | |
swamped the much weaker ones coming from the Earth. | 0:08:24 | 0:08:27 | |
So he established that pigeons can use either the sun | 0:08:29 | 0:08:33 | |
or the Earth's magnetism to find their way around, | 0:08:33 | 0:08:37 | |
according to conditions. | 0:08:37 | 0:08:38 | |
But how do the birds detect the Earth's magnetism? | 0:08:40 | 0:08:45 | |
This compass has a magnetised needle, | 0:08:48 | 0:08:51 | |
which is sensitive to the Earth's magnetic field. | 0:08:51 | 0:08:54 | |
It will always point north, | 0:08:54 | 0:08:56 | |
allowing you to check the direction that you're travelling in. | 0:08:56 | 0:08:59 | |
But what about pigeons? | 0:08:59 | 0:09:01 | |
What could they be using to detect magnetic fields? | 0:09:01 | 0:09:04 | |
Scientists were puzzled because they couldn't find | 0:09:04 | 0:09:08 | |
any cells or organs in birds that were sensitive to magnetism. | 0:09:08 | 0:09:11 | |
Then, in 2007, microscopic clusters of iron-rich cells | 0:09:14 | 0:09:19 | |
were found in the beaks of homing pigeons, | 0:09:19 | 0:09:23 | |
and it was thought that these might help them to detect magnetic forces. | 0:09:23 | 0:09:28 | |
But only five years later, new research shattered that idea. | 0:09:29 | 0:09:34 | |
It seems that the cells in fact are a defence against infection | 0:09:34 | 0:09:37 | |
and have nothing whatsoever to do with magnetism. | 0:09:37 | 0:09:41 | |
So the search for the pigeon's magnetic compass still continues. | 0:09:41 | 0:09:46 | |
But we are now beginning to understand the other aspects | 0:09:46 | 0:09:50 | |
of their navigational skills. | 0:09:50 | 0:09:52 | |
A compass is just one of the tools we need for navigation. | 0:09:55 | 0:09:59 | |
But it's not much help | 0:09:59 | 0:10:01 | |
unless you know in which direction your destination lies. | 0:10:01 | 0:10:04 | |
And to know that, you need a map of some kind. | 0:10:04 | 0:10:08 | |
One way of creating a map in your mind | 0:10:10 | 0:10:12 | |
is to memorise the prominent features of a landscape. | 0:10:12 | 0:10:15 | |
Scientists at Oxford University are now using GPS technology | 0:10:17 | 0:10:22 | |
to discover if pigeons do this. | 0:10:22 | 0:10:24 | |
This bird has a GPS transmitting device strapped to its back | 0:10:27 | 0:10:31 | |
in a specially designed backpack | 0:10:31 | 0:10:33 | |
which causes it very little discomfort. | 0:10:33 | 0:10:36 | |
And that will record the progress of the bird, second by second, | 0:10:36 | 0:10:41 | |
once I let it go. | 0:10:41 | 0:10:43 | |
It comes from a loft over there about six miles away in Oxford. | 0:10:43 | 0:10:48 | |
Let's see whether or not she heads in that direction. | 0:10:48 | 0:10:51 | |
Off you go. | 0:10:51 | 0:10:53 | |
Birds, like this, that are released in a new place | 0:10:56 | 0:10:59 | |
still ahead confidently in the direction of their home loft. | 0:10:59 | 0:11:04 | |
How do they do it? | 0:11:04 | 0:11:05 | |
Data from GPS trackers | 0:11:08 | 0:11:09 | |
and cameras attached to the backs of pigeons | 0:11:09 | 0:11:12 | |
have helped to answer that question. | 0:11:12 | 0:11:15 | |
At first, a bird released in an unfamiliar place | 0:11:16 | 0:11:20 | |
will use the sun or the Earth's magnetic field to get its bearings. | 0:11:20 | 0:11:24 | |
But if you plot its movements on a map, | 0:11:25 | 0:11:28 | |
you can see that at first it doesn't find the shortest route home. | 0:11:28 | 0:11:32 | |
Then something changes. | 0:11:34 | 0:11:35 | |
After repeated releases, the bird is able to use its experience | 0:11:37 | 0:11:41 | |
to take a more direct way back. | 0:11:41 | 0:11:43 | |
So it appears that pigeons learn to recognise features of the landscape | 0:11:48 | 0:11:52 | |
such as hedgerows and trees, or even roads and buildings. | 0:11:52 | 0:11:56 | |
They then memorise these to create a mental map of the area | 0:11:59 | 0:12:03 | |
and by following familiar landmarks, they find their way back home. | 0:12:03 | 0:12:08 | |
It seems that pigeons are creatures of habit. | 0:12:11 | 0:12:14 | |
They like to stick to the routes they know well. | 0:12:14 | 0:12:17 | |
Here are some tracks of a pigeon that lives in its home loft up here | 0:12:18 | 0:12:22 | |
and was taken repeatedly to this point here. | 0:12:22 | 0:12:25 | |
Watch what happens. | 0:12:26 | 0:12:28 | |
It consistently makes a beeline for the major A road | 0:12:30 | 0:12:33 | |
that conveniently runs past its home loft. | 0:12:33 | 0:12:36 | |
And even when released some distance from this road, | 0:12:38 | 0:12:41 | |
it will cut across to it and then follow it all the way home, | 0:12:41 | 0:12:44 | |
as shown by the red lines. | 0:12:44 | 0:12:46 | |
Some birds even fly around roundabouts | 0:12:53 | 0:12:56 | |
before choosing the exit that will lead them back. | 0:12:56 | 0:12:59 | |
So it seems that pigeons use methods of navigation | 0:13:01 | 0:13:04 | |
more like ours than we might have imagined. | 0:13:04 | 0:13:08 | |
We now know that pigeons have | 0:13:11 | 0:13:13 | |
a whole range of navigational techniques at their disposal | 0:13:13 | 0:13:16 | |
and can call on them as needed. | 0:13:16 | 0:13:18 | |
It's a remarkably sophisticated system | 0:13:19 | 0:13:22 | |
that enables them to find their way in nearly any situation. | 0:13:22 | 0:13:26 | |
We've learned a lot about the pigeon's homing skills, | 0:13:31 | 0:13:33 | |
but some mysteries still remain. | 0:13:33 | 0:13:36 | |
In 1997, over 60,000 British birds were released | 0:13:37 | 0:13:42 | |
during a pigeon race in southern France. | 0:13:42 | 0:13:44 | |
Most of them didn't make it and were never seen again. | 0:13:47 | 0:13:50 | |
In pigeon racing terms, the loss of so many birds was unheard of. | 0:13:52 | 0:13:58 | |
A disaster. | 0:13:58 | 0:13:59 | |
One bird might get lost, but tens of thousands? | 0:13:59 | 0:14:03 | |
To get back to their lofts nearly 500 miles away, | 0:14:07 | 0:14:10 | |
the pigeons had to cross the English Channel. | 0:14:10 | 0:14:14 | |
It was not an unusually long or difficult journey | 0:14:14 | 0:14:17 | |
for a racing pigeon, so why did the birds not make it? | 0:14:17 | 0:14:21 | |
Well, there was a strange coincidence. | 0:14:22 | 0:14:25 | |
At the very same time the racing pigeons were crossing the Channel, | 0:14:27 | 0:14:30 | |
a Concorde supersonic airliner was flying along the Channel | 0:14:30 | 0:14:35 | |
on its morning flight from Paris to New York. | 0:14:35 | 0:14:38 | |
The Concorde generates a shock wave almost 100 miles wide. | 0:14:41 | 0:14:44 | |
The pigeons flying below could not have escaped it. | 0:14:46 | 0:14:49 | |
Could it be that this enormous wave of sound blotted out | 0:14:49 | 0:14:53 | |
all other acoustic information? | 0:14:53 | 0:14:56 | |
We know that pigeons can hear low-frequency infra-sounds | 0:14:58 | 0:15:02 | |
such as those generated by ocean waves. | 0:15:02 | 0:15:05 | |
And these might provide them with an acoustic map of their surroundings. | 0:15:06 | 0:15:12 | |
The jury is still out as to whether pigeons really do use | 0:15:14 | 0:15:17 | |
infra-sound to navigate | 0:15:17 | 0:15:19 | |
and whether that explains the case of the disappearing pigeons. | 0:15:19 | 0:15:23 | |
But it's an intriguing possibility. | 0:15:24 | 0:15:27 | |
The familiar pigeon continues to surprise us. | 0:15:30 | 0:15:34 | |
And even today, we're a long way off from understanding | 0:15:34 | 0:15:37 | |
all its route-finding techniques. | 0:15:37 | 0:15:40 | |
The small dung beetle has a brain no larger than a grain of rice. | 0:15:42 | 0:15:47 | |
But, like the pigeon, it is capable of astonishing navigational feats. | 0:15:47 | 0:15:53 | |
Moving large loads by walking head down and backwards | 0:15:57 | 0:16:00 | |
must surely be one of the most bizarre ways | 0:16:00 | 0:16:03 | |
of collecting your food. | 0:16:03 | 0:16:04 | |
Dung beetles are well known | 0:16:07 | 0:16:08 | |
for rolling their balls of dung in this way. | 0:16:08 | 0:16:11 | |
But why do they do so? | 0:16:11 | 0:16:13 | |
And how can they see where they are going? | 0:16:13 | 0:16:16 | |
Ball rolling by dung beetles is probably | 0:16:19 | 0:16:22 | |
one of the oldest recorded accounts of animal behaviour, | 0:16:22 | 0:16:26 | |
dating back nearly 2,000 years. | 0:16:26 | 0:16:29 | |
These insects were held sacred by the ancient Egyptians, | 0:16:29 | 0:16:32 | |
and in the fifth century, an Egyptian scholar called Horapollo | 0:16:32 | 0:16:37 | |
described the dung beetle as rolling its ball from east to west. | 0:16:37 | 0:16:42 | |
It seems that he was suggesting that the beetles always | 0:16:42 | 0:16:45 | |
moved their dung in a particular direction. | 0:16:45 | 0:16:48 | |
But how true is that? | 0:16:48 | 0:16:50 | |
In the following 2,000 years, little was done to examine the question. | 0:16:50 | 0:16:55 | |
Dung beetles feed on what is, to the animals that produce it, | 0:16:57 | 0:17:01 | |
a waste product - dung. | 0:17:01 | 0:17:04 | |
But in fact, there's more than enough nourishment in one dropping | 0:17:04 | 0:17:08 | |
to sustain a great number of beetles. | 0:17:08 | 0:17:10 | |
They locate a newly dropped pile using their sense of smell. | 0:17:12 | 0:17:16 | |
Their strong and powerful legs enable them | 0:17:17 | 0:17:20 | |
to break up the dropping | 0:17:20 | 0:17:21 | |
and their mouthparts are specially shaped | 0:17:21 | 0:17:24 | |
to extract the nutrients and moisture that they need. | 0:17:24 | 0:17:27 | |
They are, in fact, one of our planet's great recyclers. | 0:17:29 | 0:17:33 | |
Dung beetles are found all over the world. | 0:17:38 | 0:17:41 | |
There are over 6,000 species and they come in all shapes and colours. | 0:17:42 | 0:17:47 | |
But they have one thing in common - | 0:17:49 | 0:17:51 | |
they all feed on dung. | 0:17:51 | 0:17:54 | |
Some of them, like this scarab beetle... | 0:17:57 | 0:17:59 | |
..have rows of projections on the head, | 0:18:01 | 0:18:04 | |
which were likened by the ancient Egyptians | 0:18:04 | 0:18:07 | |
to the rays of the rising sun. | 0:18:07 | 0:18:10 | |
The broad front legs also carry spines. | 0:18:10 | 0:18:13 | |
These are adaptations for digging and shaping dung balls, | 0:18:13 | 0:18:18 | |
while the back legs are flattened | 0:18:18 | 0:18:21 | |
and have tiny hairs for controlling the ball. | 0:18:21 | 0:18:24 | |
But most dung beetles don't roll balls. | 0:18:26 | 0:18:30 | |
They live either in the dung itself | 0:18:30 | 0:18:32 | |
or tunnel directly into the soil beneath the dropping. | 0:18:32 | 0:18:36 | |
Only 10% transport dung above ground. | 0:18:36 | 0:18:39 | |
And they do so because competition near a pile of dung | 0:18:39 | 0:18:43 | |
can be very fierce. | 0:18:43 | 0:18:45 | |
The first ones at the scene are usually the rollers. | 0:18:47 | 0:18:51 | |
They take a little lump, mould it into a ball and then start rolling. | 0:18:54 | 0:18:59 | |
But that's easier said than done. | 0:19:04 | 0:19:07 | |
Rivals are on the lookout for an easy meal | 0:19:09 | 0:19:11 | |
and will only too quickly steal a ball if they can. | 0:19:11 | 0:19:15 | |
With competition so intense, some species roll their ball away | 0:19:28 | 0:19:32 | |
as quickly as possible and then bury it. | 0:19:32 | 0:19:35 | |
They can then eat it later | 0:19:37 | 0:19:39 | |
without the risk of another stealing their meal. | 0:19:39 | 0:19:42 | |
Dung beetles appear to roll their balls of dung with great purpose. | 0:19:45 | 0:19:50 | |
But it's only recently been discovered just how determined | 0:19:50 | 0:19:54 | |
they can be to do so in one particular direction. | 0:19:54 | 0:19:58 | |
Watch what happens if I put this dung beetle | 0:19:58 | 0:20:02 | |
on this board here. | 0:20:02 | 0:20:03 | |
And then put an obstacle in its way. | 0:20:05 | 0:20:09 | |
And what does it do? | 0:20:11 | 0:20:13 | |
It's going to go around. | 0:20:14 | 0:20:16 | |
Excellent. | 0:20:16 | 0:20:17 | |
And it resumes its previous direction. | 0:20:18 | 0:20:22 | |
It's clear that the beetle knows exactly | 0:20:24 | 0:20:26 | |
in which direction it wants to travel | 0:20:26 | 0:20:29 | |
and will continue to do so even when it's deflected by obstacles. | 0:20:29 | 0:20:33 | |
Now, let's make things even more difficult | 0:20:35 | 0:20:38 | |
using this turntable. | 0:20:38 | 0:20:39 | |
Let's see which direction | 0:20:41 | 0:20:43 | |
he wants to go. | 0:20:43 | 0:20:44 | |
He's going to come towards my hand. | 0:20:46 | 0:20:48 | |
Now I'll turn the world through 90 degrees beneath his feet | 0:20:48 | 0:20:52 | |
and yet he maintains | 0:20:52 | 0:20:55 | |
the same direction. | 0:20:55 | 0:20:57 | |
Why should it want to do that? | 0:20:57 | 0:20:59 | |
It's quite extraordinary. | 0:20:59 | 0:21:02 | |
He corrects his course instantaneously. | 0:21:02 | 0:21:05 | |
How does he know how to do this, | 0:21:05 | 0:21:07 | |
and why is he so determined to travel in a set direction? | 0:21:07 | 0:21:11 | |
The answer is, in fact, quite simple. | 0:21:15 | 0:21:17 | |
The fastest way to make a getaway | 0:21:18 | 0:21:21 | |
is to roll your ball in a straight line. | 0:21:21 | 0:21:23 | |
And that is exactly what the beetles try to do. | 0:21:26 | 0:21:29 | |
Using only its front legs, | 0:21:34 | 0:21:36 | |
a beetle can move a dung ball that is up to 60 times its own weight. | 0:21:36 | 0:21:40 | |
But walking head down and backwards has its own problems. | 0:21:43 | 0:21:48 | |
You can't see where you're going... | 0:21:48 | 0:21:50 | |
..or watch out for danger. | 0:21:53 | 0:21:55 | |
With competition around the pile of dung so intense, | 0:22:10 | 0:22:13 | |
it's obviously a good thing to get away from it as quickly as possible. | 0:22:13 | 0:22:17 | |
But how do the beetles decide in which direction to go? | 0:22:17 | 0:22:21 | |
Well, it seems it's all to do with a little dance that they perform | 0:22:21 | 0:22:25 | |
on top of their dung balls. | 0:22:25 | 0:22:27 | |
Let me see if I can persuade this one to do it. | 0:22:27 | 0:22:29 | |
I'll try to get it to walk up this ramp | 0:22:32 | 0:22:35 | |
until it falls off the end | 0:22:35 | 0:22:37 | |
and is separated from its ball. | 0:22:37 | 0:22:39 | |
Watch what it does then. | 0:22:42 | 0:22:44 | |
This little dance was once | 0:22:48 | 0:22:50 | |
regarded as being a sign of happiness, | 0:22:50 | 0:22:52 | |
that the beetle was delighted to have found a dung ball. | 0:22:52 | 0:22:55 | |
But actually, it's much more than that. | 0:22:55 | 0:22:58 | |
It's a way of deciding which direction it should go. | 0:22:58 | 0:23:02 | |
And what does it use as a cue to make that decision? | 0:23:02 | 0:23:06 | |
Well, recent research is beginning to produce the answer. | 0:23:06 | 0:23:10 | |
What appears to be a dance is actually | 0:23:13 | 0:23:16 | |
a way of looking around to get its bearings. | 0:23:16 | 0:23:19 | |
But what exactly is it looking for? | 0:23:22 | 0:23:24 | |
We can test what the beetles are using for a cue | 0:23:29 | 0:23:32 | |
by fitting them with caps. | 0:23:32 | 0:23:34 | |
This cap allows the beetle to see the ground beneath, | 0:23:34 | 0:23:38 | |
but not the sky above. | 0:23:38 | 0:23:40 | |
Compare that with this one without a cap. | 0:23:40 | 0:23:43 | |
Making up its mind. | 0:23:46 | 0:23:47 | |
And away it goes. | 0:23:47 | 0:23:49 | |
This one is still baffled. | 0:23:50 | 0:23:52 | |
The beetle with the cap clearly can't decide | 0:23:55 | 0:23:57 | |
in which direction to go. | 0:23:57 | 0:23:59 | |
Such experiments show | 0:24:05 | 0:24:07 | |
that dung beetles have to see the sky in order to orientate. | 0:24:07 | 0:24:11 | |
Their eyes are split in two halves by shovel-like extensions | 0:24:14 | 0:24:19 | |
on the sides of the head. | 0:24:19 | 0:24:21 | |
So, as they roll their balls of dung, | 0:24:22 | 0:24:25 | |
the upward-facing half can watch the sky above | 0:24:25 | 0:24:29 | |
and use the sun as a celestial compass. | 0:24:29 | 0:24:32 | |
But, contrary to what that ancient Egyptian scholar claimed, | 0:24:34 | 0:24:38 | |
they don't always travel from east to west. | 0:24:38 | 0:24:41 | |
Each beetle seems to choose a particular direction | 0:24:44 | 0:24:47 | |
away from the dung heap and then keeps to that course. | 0:24:47 | 0:24:50 | |
But why should anyone suppose | 0:24:56 | 0:24:58 | |
that the dung beetle always rolls its dung from east to west? | 0:24:58 | 0:25:02 | |
Well, the ancient Egyptians believed that their sun god, Khepri, | 0:25:02 | 0:25:07 | |
was responsible for rolling the sun across the sky every day | 0:25:07 | 0:25:11 | |
in just that direction. | 0:25:11 | 0:25:13 | |
The scarab, the Egyptian dung beetle, | 0:25:14 | 0:25:17 | |
similarly rolls its dung ball from dawn to dusk | 0:25:17 | 0:25:21 | |
and came to symbolise the sun god. | 0:25:21 | 0:25:23 | |
And so the god Khepri was often depicted with a scarab for a head. | 0:25:26 | 0:25:31 | |
The Egyptians also saw the scarab | 0:25:34 | 0:25:37 | |
as a symbol of life and resurrection. | 0:25:37 | 0:25:40 | |
The adult beetle would disappear underground with the ball of dung, | 0:25:40 | 0:25:44 | |
and when the eggs hatched, shiny, new scarab beetles | 0:25:44 | 0:25:47 | |
would seem to appear magically out of nowhere. | 0:25:47 | 0:25:50 | |
So the scarab came to symbolise creation, life | 0:25:50 | 0:25:55 | |
and even rebirth. | 0:25:55 | 0:25:57 | |
It's easy to see how the sun helps dung beetles | 0:25:59 | 0:26:02 | |
to navigate during the day. | 0:26:02 | 0:26:04 | |
But some beetles are nocturnal. | 0:26:06 | 0:26:08 | |
How do they keep on a straight course? | 0:26:10 | 0:26:12 | |
On a clear night, | 0:26:19 | 0:26:20 | |
the moon and millions of stars illuminate the sky, | 0:26:20 | 0:26:24 | |
just as they appear to do in this planetarium. | 0:26:24 | 0:26:27 | |
This is a nocturnal scarab beetle. | 0:26:27 | 0:26:31 | |
And scientists have recently discovered that it can use | 0:26:31 | 0:26:34 | |
the light from the sky to navigate. | 0:26:34 | 0:26:38 | |
But there is a puzzle. | 0:26:39 | 0:26:41 | |
The scarab beetle has very, very small eyes. | 0:26:41 | 0:26:45 | |
And whereas it can certainly see the moon, | 0:26:45 | 0:26:48 | |
it can't distinguish individual stars. | 0:26:48 | 0:26:51 | |
So how then does it navigate | 0:26:51 | 0:26:53 | |
on a moonless night? | 0:26:53 | 0:26:56 | |
The brightest light in the night sky | 0:27:00 | 0:27:02 | |
comes from the great band of stars known as the Milky Way. | 0:27:02 | 0:27:06 | |
And in the southern hemisphere, where these beetles live, | 0:27:07 | 0:27:11 | |
the Milky Way is particularly vivid. | 0:27:11 | 0:27:14 | |
Researchers have discovered that, as long as the Milky Way is in view, | 0:27:17 | 0:27:20 | |
the beetle and its ball can roll along and stay on course. | 0:27:20 | 0:27:24 | |
Without this bright band of light, | 0:27:26 | 0:27:28 | |
the beetle has no reference point and is lost. | 0:27:28 | 0:27:32 | |
The revelation that the beetles used starlight to navigate | 0:27:34 | 0:27:38 | |
astonished the scientific world. | 0:27:38 | 0:27:41 | |
How extraordinary that a tiny insect could use | 0:27:41 | 0:27:44 | |
the edge of our galaxy to find its way around. | 0:27:44 | 0:27:48 | |
Had the ancient Egyptians known that, surely they would have | 0:27:48 | 0:27:52 | |
felt vindicated in giving the scarab the status of a god. | 0:27:52 | 0:27:57 | |
So, the dung beetle and the pigeon are both ordinary creatures | 0:28:00 | 0:28:05 | |
that have found extraordinary solutions | 0:28:05 | 0:28:08 | |
to the problem that faces us all - | 0:28:08 | 0:28:11 | |
how to find the way. | 0:28:11 | 0:28:14 |