Browse content similar to Prairie Dogs - Talk of the Town. Check below for episodes and series from the same categories and more!
Line | From | To | |
---|---|---|---|
The North American prairies, home to the bison. | 0:00:16 | 0:00:21 | |
But this film isn't about the bison. | 0:00:23 | 0:00:25 | |
It's about a tiny creature that lives beneath the bison's feet... | 0:00:25 | 0:00:29 | |
..the prairie dog. | 0:00:32 | 0:00:35 | |
You might easily overlook the prairie dog, | 0:00:36 | 0:00:39 | |
until it opens its mouth. | 0:00:39 | 0:00:41 | |
SQUEAKING | 0:00:41 | 0:00:43 | |
It's a little animal with a big voice. | 0:00:43 | 0:00:49 | |
A team of scientists believe this noise | 0:00:49 | 0:00:52 | |
is in fact a very complex language, | 0:00:52 | 0:00:55 | |
which is remarkable for an animal many see as lowly vermin. | 0:00:55 | 0:01:00 | |
So, what are they saying? | 0:01:01 | 0:01:04 | |
To find out, scientists have devised a set of cunning experiments. | 0:01:04 | 0:01:10 | |
But could these barks, squeaks and chirps really be a language? | 0:01:13 | 0:01:19 | |
To our human ears, this just sounds like a lot of unnecessary noise, | 0:01:31 | 0:01:36 | |
but to other prairie dogs it's all part of a jolly good conversation. | 0:01:36 | 0:01:42 | |
That is, if you believe this man, | 0:01:42 | 0:01:44 | |
prairie dog expert Professor Con Slobodchikoff. | 0:01:44 | 0:01:48 | |
Prairie dogs have the most complex natural language | 0:01:48 | 0:01:53 | |
that's been decoded so far. | 0:01:53 | 0:01:55 | |
They have words for different predators, they have | 0:01:55 | 0:01:59 | |
descriptive words for describing the individual features of predators, | 0:01:59 | 0:02:05 | |
so it's a pretty complex language that has a lot of elements. | 0:02:05 | 0:02:10 | |
A relative of the squirrel | 0:02:10 | 0:02:12 | |
with a language second only to humans in its complexity? | 0:02:12 | 0:02:16 | |
That's a bold claim. | 0:02:16 | 0:02:19 | |
But Con has a problem. | 0:02:19 | 0:02:21 | |
He's fighting an in-built prejudice against these little rodents. | 0:02:21 | 0:02:25 | |
Convincing the wider world that prairie dogs are great linguists | 0:02:25 | 0:02:29 | |
is proving a tough challenge. | 0:02:29 | 0:02:32 | |
He believes if we just get to know them a little better, | 0:02:32 | 0:02:35 | |
understand their world, then we might just see his point of view. | 0:02:35 | 0:02:41 | |
Life as a prairie dog is complicated. | 0:02:41 | 0:02:45 | |
It's not just a simple matter | 0:02:45 | 0:02:47 | |
of eating grass and minding your own business. | 0:02:47 | 0:02:50 | |
You have to share your space with lots of other prairie dogs. | 0:02:52 | 0:02:55 | |
Every prairie dog is part of a big community called a town. | 0:02:57 | 0:03:02 | |
The largest prairie dog towns stretch over tens of miles, | 0:03:04 | 0:03:08 | |
their excavated homes pockmarking the prairie landscape. | 0:03:08 | 0:03:12 | |
Living together requires impeccable social skills. | 0:03:15 | 0:03:19 | |
Kissing is all the rage in prairie dog town. | 0:03:19 | 0:03:23 | |
A little taste of saliva | 0:03:23 | 0:03:25 | |
is like a calling card that helps identify a visitor. | 0:03:25 | 0:03:28 | |
Prairie dogs, though, are nervous creatures, | 0:03:30 | 0:03:33 | |
always on the lookout for signs of trouble across town. | 0:03:33 | 0:03:36 | |
But what is there to worry about? | 0:03:38 | 0:03:41 | |
Prairie dogs aren't the only animals living in the town. | 0:03:43 | 0:03:47 | |
Squatters have moved into some of their vacant burrows. | 0:03:49 | 0:03:53 | |
All these strangers make prairie dogs a little nervous. | 0:04:01 | 0:04:05 | |
And so they should be. | 0:04:05 | 0:04:07 | |
Prairie dogs are under attack from all quarters. | 0:04:08 | 0:04:12 | |
There are times when they must feel besieged | 0:04:12 | 0:04:15 | |
by animals that want to eat them. | 0:04:15 | 0:04:16 | |
Even when they retreat to their underground burrows, | 0:04:18 | 0:04:21 | |
the prairie dogs can't really relax. | 0:04:21 | 0:04:23 | |
With no safety anywhere, above or below ground, prairie dogs | 0:04:35 | 0:04:40 | |
have come up with a clever strategy for dealing with these threats. | 0:04:40 | 0:04:43 | |
LOUD SQUEAKING | 0:04:43 | 0:04:45 | |
They shout. | 0:04:45 | 0:04:47 | |
Very loudly. | 0:04:47 | 0:04:50 | |
Technically speaking, it's an alarm call. | 0:04:50 | 0:04:55 | |
You might still be wondering why Con would choose | 0:04:58 | 0:05:01 | |
such a humble subject to study animal language. | 0:05:01 | 0:05:04 | |
After all, there are more likely animal linguists - | 0:05:04 | 0:05:07 | |
whales or monkeys. | 0:05:07 | 0:05:10 | |
But there was something about those prairie dog alarm calls | 0:05:10 | 0:05:14 | |
that piqued his interest. | 0:05:14 | 0:05:17 | |
With so many predators living in prairie dog town, Con wondered, | 0:05:17 | 0:05:20 | |
do the prairie dogs have just one call, | 0:05:20 | 0:05:23 | |
or do they have different calls for the different predators? | 0:05:23 | 0:05:27 | |
There's only one way to find out. | 0:05:27 | 0:05:30 | |
Con must test their calls. | 0:05:30 | 0:05:34 | |
He's on his way to a remote ranch in the north of New Mexico. | 0:05:34 | 0:05:39 | |
It's a haven for prairie dogs. | 0:05:39 | 0:05:42 | |
Two species live here, Gunnison's and black-tails. | 0:05:42 | 0:05:46 | |
Joining him are two of his students, Patricia Dennis, | 0:05:46 | 0:05:49 | |
who will help record the calls, | 0:05:49 | 0:05:51 | |
and Bill Briggs, who's devised a series of experiments | 0:05:51 | 0:05:55 | |
to test the prairie dogs. | 0:05:55 | 0:05:57 | |
Together, this team hopes to show once and for all | 0:06:00 | 0:06:03 | |
that we have underestimated the little prairie dog. | 0:06:03 | 0:06:07 | |
First of all, Con looks for signs of predators in his research area. | 0:06:18 | 0:06:23 | |
Here's a badger excavation. | 0:06:23 | 0:06:24 | |
This is very fresh. | 0:06:24 | 0:06:26 | |
It probably happened in the last day or two. | 0:06:26 | 0:06:29 | |
This is the way that badgers typically hunt. | 0:06:29 | 0:06:32 | |
They'll excavate a prairie dog burrow and then go inside, | 0:06:32 | 0:06:35 | |
and while the prairie dogs are sleeping, | 0:06:35 | 0:06:37 | |
they will eat the prairie dogs | 0:06:37 | 0:06:39 | |
and then they'll also sleep inside the burrow. | 0:06:39 | 0:06:42 | |
They typically excavate a number of burrows | 0:06:42 | 0:06:45 | |
all around us in a particular area, and then they move on | 0:06:45 | 0:06:48 | |
to another place once they've eaten all the prairie dogs | 0:06:48 | 0:06:51 | |
that live in a particular burrow system. | 0:06:51 | 0:06:54 | |
The presence of predators is welcome news for Con. | 0:06:57 | 0:07:00 | |
The prairie dogs should be on a state of alert, | 0:07:00 | 0:07:04 | |
and that'll mean good alarm calls. | 0:07:04 | 0:07:06 | |
SQUEAKING | 0:07:06 | 0:07:08 | |
But waiting for predators to appear would be far too time-consuming. | 0:07:08 | 0:07:13 | |
These imaginative scientists have a solution - | 0:07:13 | 0:07:16 | |
they bring their own. | 0:07:16 | 0:07:17 | |
Time to set up the first experiment. | 0:07:34 | 0:07:38 | |
Bill must keep the dummy predator under wraps | 0:07:39 | 0:07:42 | |
while he finds a suitable spot. | 0:07:42 | 0:07:44 | |
Some prairie dogs are already a little spooked. | 0:07:46 | 0:07:50 | |
Con uses his car as a screen to hide behind. | 0:08:06 | 0:08:09 | |
It's important that no-one is visible. | 0:08:09 | 0:08:11 | |
The predator is revealed... | 0:08:15 | 0:08:18 | |
..a plastic coyote without any legs. | 0:08:21 | 0:08:24 | |
It seems like an encouraging result. | 0:08:29 | 0:08:31 | |
The prairie dogs are certainly being vocal. | 0:08:31 | 0:08:33 | |
What's really wonderful about this is that other individuals are chiming in | 0:08:36 | 0:08:42 | |
just in response to the vocalisation, | 0:08:42 | 0:08:44 | |
so they don't even have to see the stimulus | 0:08:44 | 0:08:47 | |
in order to know that this is a coyote, and they start yelling. | 0:08:47 | 0:08:50 | |
Every call recorded must subsequently be analysed by Con. | 0:08:52 | 0:08:56 | |
We can hear the difference in the calls | 0:08:56 | 0:08:59 | |
between the different predators, but that's not really enough for us. | 0:08:59 | 0:09:03 | |
What we really need is some sort of analytical tools | 0:09:03 | 0:09:07 | |
for measuring what the frequency and time elements in the call are. | 0:09:07 | 0:09:11 | |
And so this software here allows us to do this. | 0:09:11 | 0:09:15 | |
This is our alarm call elicited for a coyote, | 0:09:17 | 0:09:21 | |
and we can hear that it has a series of rhythmic chirps. | 0:09:21 | 0:09:26 | |
We think that each prairie dog | 0:09:26 | 0:09:28 | |
has its own individual, unique tonal qualities, | 0:09:28 | 0:09:31 | |
just like humans have different voices and different voice prints. | 0:09:31 | 0:09:36 | |
But even though each individual prairie dog might differ, | 0:09:36 | 0:09:39 | |
all the prairie dogs have a common call | 0:09:39 | 0:09:41 | |
for a coyote even within those individual voice print differences. | 0:09:41 | 0:09:46 | |
So we think that when a prairie dog is calling for a coyote, | 0:09:46 | 0:09:49 | |
they probably know who the prairie dog is that's calling, | 0:09:49 | 0:09:53 | |
just the same way that we can on the telephone hear somebody's voice | 0:09:53 | 0:09:57 | |
and immediately recognise it as a person that we know. | 0:09:57 | 0:10:01 | |
SQUEAKY CHIRPING | 0:10:01 | 0:10:02 | |
Everything is ready for experiment number two, the stuffed badger. | 0:10:05 | 0:10:09 | |
Con and his team are very excited by this one. | 0:10:18 | 0:10:22 | |
This is the first time they've ever tested a badger. | 0:10:22 | 0:10:24 | |
Will the prairie dogs have a specific alarm call for it? | 0:10:26 | 0:10:30 | |
The badger's out. | 0:10:40 | 0:10:42 | |
LOUD SQUEAKING | 0:10:42 | 0:10:45 | |
Well, the prairie dogs are certainly calling, | 0:10:55 | 0:10:58 | |
but are these calls different from the earlier ones to the coyote? | 0:10:58 | 0:11:03 | |
Con hopes so. | 0:11:03 | 0:11:05 | |
Now the researchers must take a look at all the recordings | 0:11:07 | 0:11:10 | |
from today's experiments. | 0:11:10 | 0:11:12 | |
The badger call is an entirely new call for us. | 0:11:12 | 0:11:14 | |
We've never recorded a call for a badger. | 0:11:14 | 0:11:17 | |
So it's really exciting to look at the structure of this call | 0:11:17 | 0:11:21 | |
and see what it looks like. | 0:11:21 | 0:11:22 | |
And it looks very different | 0:11:22 | 0:11:24 | |
from any of the other calls that we've recorded. | 0:11:24 | 0:11:27 | |
SQUEAKING | 0:11:27 | 0:11:28 | |
SQUEAKING | 0:11:30 | 0:11:32 | |
HIGHER-PITCHED SQUEAKING | 0:11:35 | 0:11:37 | |
To Con's expert ears and eyes, there is a difference both in their calls | 0:11:37 | 0:11:42 | |
and in the shape of their sound graphs. | 0:11:42 | 0:11:45 | |
The shape of each sound curve | 0:11:49 | 0:11:51 | |
varies subtly, but is consistent for each predator. | 0:11:51 | 0:11:54 | |
But to convince any would-be doubters, | 0:12:00 | 0:12:03 | |
Con has another experiment planned. | 0:12:03 | 0:12:05 | |
Over his 30 years of research, he's noticed that prairie dogs | 0:12:08 | 0:12:11 | |
react in different ways to different predators. | 0:12:11 | 0:12:16 | |
If the alarm calls for coyote and badger do indeed differ, | 0:12:16 | 0:12:19 | |
then playing back the two calls | 0:12:19 | 0:12:21 | |
to a group of prairie dogs should produce distinct responses. | 0:12:21 | 0:12:27 | |
Bill's big speakers get fired up. | 0:12:27 | 0:12:29 | |
HIGH-PITCHED SQUEAKING | 0:12:29 | 0:12:31 | |
First, the alarm call for coyote. | 0:12:32 | 0:12:36 | |
The prairie dogs react instantly. | 0:12:36 | 0:12:39 | |
Having spooked one part of the colony with the loudest | 0:12:47 | 0:12:50 | |
alarm call they've ever heard, | 0:12:50 | 0:12:53 | |
the team moves on to an undisturbed spot | 0:12:53 | 0:12:56 | |
to try out their second alarm call, the badger. | 0:12:56 | 0:13:00 | |
SQUEAKING | 0:13:04 | 0:13:05 | |
The reaction is very different. | 0:13:09 | 0:13:12 | |
We had a terrific response, and for the coyote call | 0:13:19 | 0:13:22 | |
they did just what they're supposed to do for a coyote. | 0:13:22 | 0:13:25 | |
They ran to the burrows and stood at the lip of the burrows and watched. | 0:13:25 | 0:13:30 | |
For the badger call, they ran to the lip of the burrows | 0:13:30 | 0:13:32 | |
and crouched down, just like they're supposed to for a badger. | 0:13:32 | 0:13:36 | |
So it was a terrific result. | 0:13:36 | 0:13:38 | |
Adopting different tactics for different predators | 0:13:38 | 0:13:42 | |
makes perfect sense. | 0:13:42 | 0:13:43 | |
A coyote will hunt by surprise, | 0:13:43 | 0:13:46 | |
so perhaps it's safest to sit up and keep an eye on it at all times. | 0:13:46 | 0:13:51 | |
Badgers are the bulldozers of the predator world. | 0:13:53 | 0:13:56 | |
They will dig into a prairie dog hole. | 0:13:56 | 0:14:00 | |
Crouching down might be a way of hiding yourself | 0:14:00 | 0:14:03 | |
so the badger doesn't choose your burrow. | 0:14:03 | 0:14:05 | |
It's been a good day for the team. | 0:14:15 | 0:14:17 | |
Every new alarm call is a new addition | 0:14:17 | 0:14:20 | |
to their dictionary of prairie dog words. | 0:14:20 | 0:14:23 | |
But there's still a long way to go | 0:14:25 | 0:14:27 | |
in their quest to prove that prairie dogs have a complex language. | 0:14:27 | 0:14:32 | |
It's early morning, and the prairie dogs are already up. | 0:14:49 | 0:14:53 | |
Con and his team are back, and they're keen to try a new test, | 0:14:57 | 0:15:01 | |
one that's a lot stranger than their earlier experiments. | 0:15:01 | 0:15:05 | |
Con has a hunch that prairie dogs have unique calls for colours. | 0:15:08 | 0:15:12 | |
It sounds a bit mad, but he's putting it to the test. | 0:15:19 | 0:15:23 | |
(Patricia here's doing a coloured shirt experiment. | 0:15:24 | 0:15:28 | |
(The idea here is to test the ability of the prairie dogs | 0:15:28 | 0:15:32 | |
(to discriminate between different colours | 0:15:32 | 0:15:35 | |
(and how they incorporate that information into their alarm calls.) | 0:15:35 | 0:15:39 | |
No joy. The prairie dogs aren't calling. | 0:15:43 | 0:15:47 | |
(I think one problem is that Patricia's not threatening enough. | 0:15:50 | 0:15:55 | |
(She tends to walk slowly and is kind of relaxed about it. | 0:15:55 | 0:15:59 | |
(I wonder if we could get Bill to wear a shirt.) | 0:15:59 | 0:16:01 | |
Time to substitute Bill for Patricia. | 0:16:03 | 0:16:06 | |
Bill's manly walk provokes a response. | 0:16:09 | 0:16:11 | |
SQUEAKING | 0:16:11 | 0:16:13 | |
But behind this quirky experiment is a bigger question. | 0:16:14 | 0:16:18 | |
Could the prairie dog calls actually contain more information | 0:16:18 | 0:16:22 | |
than just the identity of the threat? | 0:16:22 | 0:16:24 | |
Could they be describing precisely what they see? | 0:16:24 | 0:16:28 | |
Con believes the answer is yes. | 0:16:28 | 0:16:30 | |
Throughout his 30-year study, he's found that changing the speed, | 0:16:32 | 0:16:36 | |
direction and even the colour of a threat | 0:16:36 | 0:16:39 | |
results in a change in the prairie dogs' alarm call. | 0:16:39 | 0:16:42 | |
We think that each one of these chirps contains a noun-like word | 0:16:46 | 0:16:50 | |
and it also contains adjective-like words, | 0:16:50 | 0:16:53 | |
which describe the colour, the size, and all of that is | 0:16:53 | 0:16:57 | |
put together into one of these single chirps, | 0:16:57 | 0:17:00 | |
pretty much the same way that we would put together a sentence. | 0:17:00 | 0:17:04 | |
So, we might say, "A tall, skinny coyote with yellow fur," | 0:17:04 | 0:17:11 | |
and the prairie dogs manage to put all of this into a single chirp. | 0:17:11 | 0:17:17 | |
In other words, prairie dogs can cram into one short call | 0:17:17 | 0:17:21 | |
the same amount of information found in a human sentence. | 0:17:21 | 0:17:25 | |
How do they do it? | 0:17:25 | 0:17:27 | |
Con suspects the answer lies in the fine detail of their calls. | 0:17:27 | 0:17:32 | |
Each alarm call or each chirp is a series of stacked harmonics, | 0:17:33 | 0:17:39 | |
that you can see one on top of the other, | 0:17:39 | 0:17:42 | |
but instead of being actual multiples of the base frequency, | 0:17:42 | 0:17:46 | |
they're actually modulated by the vocal apparatus of the prairie dog, | 0:17:46 | 0:17:51 | |
and that's what contains the information that this is a coyote | 0:17:51 | 0:17:55 | |
as well as the general colour and size and shape of the coyote. | 0:17:55 | 0:17:58 | |
So prairie dogs can subtly alter the meaning of their chirp | 0:18:00 | 0:18:04 | |
by varying each harmonic. | 0:18:04 | 0:18:06 | |
It's an amazing way of tightly packaging information. | 0:18:08 | 0:18:11 | |
All these findings demonstrate | 0:18:15 | 0:18:17 | |
this is an animal we have grossly underestimated, | 0:18:17 | 0:18:20 | |
that the prairie dog is much more complex than we thought. | 0:18:20 | 0:18:25 | |
This is no surprise to the man | 0:18:25 | 0:18:27 | |
who has doggedly championed prairie dogs his entire career. | 0:18:27 | 0:18:30 | |
Everything about prairie dogs points to them being complex animals - | 0:18:33 | 0:18:38 | |
even their homes. | 0:18:38 | 0:18:39 | |
This is a prairie dog hole. | 0:18:41 | 0:18:42 | |
It is part of an elaborate burrow system that is underground. | 0:18:42 | 0:18:48 | |
The hole goes down maybe about between one and three metres, | 0:18:48 | 0:18:53 | |
and then the entire burrow system | 0:18:53 | 0:18:55 | |
stretches out for anywhere between three and five metres, | 0:18:55 | 0:19:00 | |
with a fairly elaborate structure. | 0:19:00 | 0:19:03 | |
There's a bathroom chamber where they pee and poop | 0:19:03 | 0:19:06 | |
and there's a nest chamber where they sleep. | 0:19:06 | 0:19:09 | |
There's also side chambers, where one prairie dog coming down | 0:19:11 | 0:19:15 | |
meets another prairie dog coming up, | 0:19:15 | 0:19:18 | |
and one of them backs into a side chamber | 0:19:18 | 0:19:20 | |
so that the other one can go past, | 0:19:20 | 0:19:22 | |
just like two cars on a narrow, winding mountain road. | 0:19:22 | 0:19:26 | |
The prairie dog burrow system | 0:19:29 | 0:19:31 | |
is remarkably sophisticated in its engineering. | 0:19:31 | 0:19:34 | |
These rodents can excavate an extensive | 0:19:34 | 0:19:37 | |
tunnel network that connects as many as six entrance holes. | 0:19:37 | 0:19:41 | |
And it's deep underground where the pups are born in spring. | 0:19:48 | 0:19:52 | |
Blind and without fur, they huddle together for warmth and security. | 0:19:56 | 0:20:01 | |
This underground world of prairie dogs is rarely observed. | 0:20:04 | 0:20:08 | |
It will be five weeks | 0:20:30 | 0:20:31 | |
before the pups are ready to leave their nursery chamber. | 0:20:31 | 0:20:35 | |
When that moment arrives, Con will be there. | 0:20:35 | 0:20:38 | |
The pup calls will be crucial to his work. | 0:20:38 | 0:20:42 | |
Above ground, the mounds are designed | 0:20:48 | 0:20:50 | |
to funnel rain away from the holes and so keep their burrows dry. | 0:20:50 | 0:20:55 | |
The hard work by these mining rodents | 0:20:57 | 0:20:59 | |
has created a vast infrastructure | 0:20:59 | 0:21:01 | |
that makes the prairies attractive for other animals. | 0:21:01 | 0:21:05 | |
Many different species of all shapes and sizes | 0:21:06 | 0:21:09 | |
have come to rely on prairie dogs for their lodging. | 0:21:09 | 0:21:12 | |
Only recently have we come to recognise that without prairie dogs, | 0:21:17 | 0:21:22 | |
the prairies would be lost for ever. | 0:21:22 | 0:21:24 | |
They're important to all life on the prairies, | 0:21:24 | 0:21:29 | |
including lots of animals that want to eat them. | 0:21:29 | 0:21:35 | |
Con believes this relationship with other animals is the main reason | 0:21:35 | 0:21:40 | |
why prairie dogs have developed a complex language. | 0:21:40 | 0:21:44 | |
So what happens to the language if you take away all the predators? | 0:21:44 | 0:21:48 | |
There's only one place where he can find the answer to that... | 0:21:50 | 0:21:54 | |
..at a zoo. | 0:21:58 | 0:22:01 | |
These prairie dogs have been in captivity all their lives. | 0:22:05 | 0:22:09 | |
They've never seen predators. | 0:22:09 | 0:22:11 | |
So what will happen when Bill and Patricia introduce | 0:22:20 | 0:22:24 | |
their dummy predators? | 0:22:24 | 0:22:26 | |
Will these captive prairie dogs | 0:22:30 | 0:22:32 | |
have a call for threats they've never seen before? | 0:22:32 | 0:22:35 | |
The coyote makes its entrance. | 0:22:42 | 0:22:46 | |
SQUEAKING | 0:22:50 | 0:22:52 | |
Well, they're certainly calling. | 0:22:53 | 0:22:55 | |
But are they saying, "Coyote"? | 0:22:55 | 0:22:58 | |
Right now, we've got about eight mics in here to make sure that we've got | 0:22:58 | 0:23:02 | |
no problems with echoes or any other aberrations, | 0:23:02 | 0:23:05 | |
and we're recording the prairie dogs' call to a coyote. | 0:23:05 | 0:23:08 | |
And I'm watching here on the computer screen to look at the spectrograph | 0:23:08 | 0:23:12 | |
to see how it's formed. And to tell you the truth, | 0:23:12 | 0:23:16 | |
it's completely different than what we're used to. | 0:23:16 | 0:23:19 | |
We still have individual chirps, but they have no definition to them. | 0:23:19 | 0:23:23 | |
They're completely different, and it's really surprising to see. | 0:23:23 | 0:23:27 | |
They're making a noise, | 0:23:27 | 0:23:29 | |
but it doesn't sound like a prairie dog word. | 0:23:29 | 0:23:32 | |
Now it's the badger's turn. | 0:23:34 | 0:23:37 | |
Is he doing anything or just looking at it? | 0:23:42 | 0:23:45 | |
There's two prairie dogs right now calling. | 0:23:45 | 0:23:47 | |
There's one calling at the badger | 0:23:47 | 0:23:49 | |
and another prairie dog by the metal grate. | 0:23:49 | 0:23:52 | |
I've got a badger sitting out there, | 0:23:55 | 0:23:58 | |
and we've got two prairie dogs calling for it. | 0:23:58 | 0:24:01 | |
And the spectrograph is just amazing. | 0:24:01 | 0:24:03 | |
It's nothing like we've seen before. | 0:24:03 | 0:24:06 | |
We've got the chevrons, but there's no structure to them. | 0:24:06 | 0:24:09 | |
Their calls are like a scream. | 0:24:10 | 0:24:13 | |
It's an alarm, but without any meaning. | 0:24:13 | 0:24:15 | |
It suggests they only develop their words | 0:24:15 | 0:24:18 | |
after repeated exposure to predators. | 0:24:18 | 0:24:21 | |
If so, that's learning, | 0:24:23 | 0:24:25 | |
and that's a key characteristic of a proper language. | 0:24:25 | 0:24:29 | |
Bill and Patricia's results from the zoo | 0:24:32 | 0:24:35 | |
show that animals who have been in a zoo setting for at least nine years, | 0:24:35 | 0:24:40 | |
who have not heard any predator calls, | 0:24:40 | 0:24:43 | |
probably lose their predator calls or lose the ability to make those calls. | 0:24:43 | 0:24:49 | |
The zoo animals have very non-specific noise-like calls, | 0:24:49 | 0:24:54 | |
and so I think that this shows that learning is important | 0:24:54 | 0:24:58 | |
in this animal language. | 0:24:58 | 0:25:00 | |
This is a major discovery. | 0:25:00 | 0:25:02 | |
Learning is a crucial element of every language. | 0:25:02 | 0:25:06 | |
But can Con build on the zoo results | 0:25:06 | 0:25:09 | |
and demonstrate learning is taking place out on the prairies? | 0:25:09 | 0:25:13 | |
If prairie dogs are learning their calls, | 0:25:13 | 0:25:16 | |
then it must be happening once the pups come above ground. | 0:25:16 | 0:25:20 | |
This, after all, is the first time they'll see other animals. | 0:25:20 | 0:25:25 | |
Testing their ability to learn will be the most important experiment | 0:25:25 | 0:25:29 | |
of all to prove that prairie dogs have a complex language. | 0:25:29 | 0:25:34 | |
Con's reputation and the reputation of all prairie dogs | 0:25:34 | 0:25:39 | |
now depends on the pups. | 0:25:39 | 0:25:41 | |
In early June, | 0:25:44 | 0:25:45 | |
the pups are just taking their very first steps above ground. | 0:25:45 | 0:25:49 | |
Bill is setting up an experiment with a new fake predator. | 0:25:52 | 0:25:57 | |
This is a silhouette of an airborne predator, like a hawk. | 0:26:06 | 0:26:11 | |
And we'll shoot this down this line to try and simulate a hawk attacking | 0:26:11 | 0:26:17 | |
a prairie dog in the colony, and it should be pretty exciting to see. | 0:26:17 | 0:26:20 | |
Well, this is pretty disappointing. | 0:26:47 | 0:26:49 | |
We've done this experiment before in the past, | 0:26:49 | 0:26:52 | |
and the animals at other sites | 0:26:52 | 0:26:54 | |
have given a characteristic hawk call and then they run to their burrows. | 0:26:54 | 0:26:58 | |
But here, they just simply run to their burrows | 0:26:58 | 0:27:01 | |
and they don't give a call. | 0:27:01 | 0:27:02 | |
Maybe they don't see any aerial predators | 0:27:02 | 0:27:05 | |
and so have no reason to give a hawk call. | 0:27:05 | 0:27:07 | |
But nevertheless, it's really disappointing. | 0:27:07 | 0:27:11 | |
Perhaps prairie dog pups are a lot cannier | 0:27:11 | 0:27:13 | |
than we give them credit for. | 0:27:13 | 0:27:15 | |
Maybe Bill's fake hawk wasn't quite realistic enough for them. | 0:27:15 | 0:27:20 | |
This is more like it. | 0:27:32 | 0:27:35 | |
A swooping hawk | 0:27:35 | 0:27:36 | |
is enough to create a state of panic in prairie dog town. | 0:27:36 | 0:27:40 | |
Many prairie dogs will go underground. | 0:27:47 | 0:27:50 | |
If the aerial approach isn't successful, | 0:27:57 | 0:28:00 | |
then the hawk has another strategy - | 0:28:00 | 0:28:03 | |
just sit and wait. | 0:28:03 | 0:28:05 | |
And wait. | 0:28:11 | 0:28:13 | |
The one thing you don't want to do is go above ground, | 0:28:15 | 0:28:19 | |
not when there's a big hawk outside. | 0:28:19 | 0:28:22 | |
But pups can be a little impatient. | 0:28:22 | 0:28:27 | |
A hawk can hold prairie dogs hostage like this for many hours. | 0:28:27 | 0:28:33 | |
Both the prairie dogs and the hawk want to feed. | 0:28:33 | 0:28:37 | |
It becomes a battle of wills. | 0:28:37 | 0:28:40 | |
Eventually, the pups claim victory. | 0:29:03 | 0:29:05 | |
Back at his study colony, | 0:29:23 | 0:29:25 | |
Con is worried that there aren't as many predators as he thought, | 0:29:25 | 0:29:29 | |
and that's making it difficult to record pup calls. | 0:29:29 | 0:29:32 | |
He needs to get the colony on a higher state of alert, | 0:29:35 | 0:29:39 | |
so he has a plan to stir things up a bit. | 0:29:39 | 0:29:42 | |
-Hi, Brent. -Hey, Con. How you doing? | 0:29:52 | 0:29:54 | |
-It's good to see you. -Good to see you. | 0:29:54 | 0:29:56 | |
-So, these are your dogs? -Yeah, these are my two dogs. | 0:29:56 | 0:29:58 | |
And when they're not chewing on each other, | 0:29:58 | 0:30:01 | |
they like to go after varmints like prairie dogs! | 0:30:01 | 0:30:03 | |
The purpose of this is to get the prairie dogs | 0:30:08 | 0:30:11 | |
more used to having predators around. | 0:30:11 | 0:30:14 | |
This colony probably has not had very much predation, | 0:30:14 | 0:30:18 | |
and they aren't calling as much as I would like. | 0:30:18 | 0:30:21 | |
And so having the dogs running around gets them more alert | 0:30:21 | 0:30:25 | |
and gets them to call more for other stimuli, | 0:30:25 | 0:30:28 | |
like some of the other experiments that we're going to do. | 0:30:28 | 0:30:31 | |
This isn't the first time Con has introduced prairie dog to real dog. | 0:30:33 | 0:30:39 | |
What we've found in the past is that they do recognise individual dogs. | 0:30:39 | 0:30:43 | |
They can describe the shape of the dogs, | 0:30:43 | 0:30:46 | |
the size of the dogs and the coat colour of the dogs. | 0:30:46 | 0:30:50 | |
But have the dogs had the desired effect? | 0:30:50 | 0:30:53 | |
Have they stirred up this colony? | 0:30:53 | 0:30:56 | |
Only one way to find out. | 0:30:56 | 0:31:00 | |
The pups seem to be on high alert, but will they call? | 0:31:14 | 0:31:19 | |
Everything is in place. | 0:31:28 | 0:31:30 | |
Now it's all down to the pups. | 0:31:30 | 0:31:33 | |
The tension is becoming unbearable. | 0:31:35 | 0:31:38 | |
SQUEAKING | 0:31:54 | 0:31:56 | |
The plastic coyote has worked its magic again. | 0:31:57 | 0:32:00 | |
But are the pups actually saying, "Coyote"? | 0:32:00 | 0:32:06 | |
If, as Con suspects, the pups learn the alarm calls, | 0:32:06 | 0:32:10 | |
then at this early stage of their lives, | 0:32:10 | 0:32:12 | |
he wouldn't expect them to know the calls for different predators. | 0:32:12 | 0:32:16 | |
Every call recorded | 0:32:18 | 0:32:20 | |
will be subjected to stringent scientific analysis. | 0:32:20 | 0:32:23 | |
Look at the difference. | 0:32:26 | 0:32:28 | |
But at first glance, the findings look positive for Con. | 0:32:28 | 0:32:32 | |
Now, he does have the same number of calls per series, | 0:32:32 | 0:32:35 | |
it seems like, and the loudness and everything else is the same. | 0:32:35 | 0:32:38 | |
And maybe the loudness and the number of calls is something that is innate, | 0:32:38 | 0:32:43 | |
and maybe the structure is something... | 0:32:43 | 0:32:45 | |
Side by side, the adult and pup calls are very different. | 0:32:45 | 0:32:49 | |
SQUEAKING | 0:32:49 | 0:32:50 | |
We have here a pup | 0:32:50 | 0:32:52 | |
and an adult calling for the coyote. | 0:32:52 | 0:32:55 | |
They sound very different, | 0:32:55 | 0:32:57 | |
and they also look very different on the computer. | 0:32:57 | 0:33:01 | |
We can see here the adult call has a lot of structure in it | 0:33:07 | 0:33:10 | |
and it contains a lot of information. | 0:33:10 | 0:33:13 | |
The pup call, on the other hand, doesn't have very much information | 0:33:13 | 0:33:17 | |
in it and it changes in structure, just like babbling in little kids. | 0:33:17 | 0:33:23 | |
So this pup is gradually acquiring more and more structure. | 0:33:23 | 0:33:27 | |
As in any language, | 0:33:27 | 0:33:28 | |
the youngsters must practise getting the words right. | 0:33:28 | 0:33:32 | |
In human babies we call it babbling, | 0:33:32 | 0:33:35 | |
and it appears that prairie dog babies babble, too, | 0:33:35 | 0:33:38 | |
as they struggle to make the right call. | 0:33:38 | 0:33:41 | |
These results are good news for Con, | 0:33:41 | 0:33:43 | |
but they don't prove they're learning, not yet. | 0:33:43 | 0:33:47 | |
To really show that, they'll need to test the same pups later in the year | 0:33:47 | 0:33:51 | |
to see if their calls change over time. | 0:33:51 | 0:33:55 | |
For now, there's nothing the team can do | 0:33:55 | 0:33:58 | |
but wait for the pups to grow up. | 0:33:58 | 0:33:59 | |
That is, if they're able to grow up. | 0:33:59 | 0:34:04 | |
Prairie dog pups are an ideal snack for hungry predators. | 0:34:06 | 0:34:11 | |
You might think the alarm calls of prairie dogs would make | 0:34:13 | 0:34:16 | |
it particularly challenging for a predator to hunt them. | 0:34:16 | 0:34:19 | |
But that doesn't seem to worry this family of swift foxes. | 0:34:19 | 0:34:23 | |
They've made a home in a deserted prairie dog burrow | 0:34:29 | 0:34:33 | |
right in the middle of a colony. | 0:34:33 | 0:34:35 | |
The fox cubs aren't even attempting to keep a low profile. | 0:34:36 | 0:34:40 | |
It's almost as if they're deliberately taunting | 0:34:42 | 0:34:45 | |
their prairie dog neighbours. | 0:34:45 | 0:34:47 | |
With four energetic cubs, Mum has a busy time. | 0:34:54 | 0:34:59 | |
She must hunt regularly. | 0:34:59 | 0:35:02 | |
When she goes out hunting, | 0:35:10 | 0:35:12 | |
the prairie dogs soon sit up and take notice. | 0:35:12 | 0:35:15 | |
But they're remarkably quiet. | 0:35:23 | 0:35:26 | |
Alarm calls warn of a pending threat, | 0:35:26 | 0:35:29 | |
but if that threat comes too close, prairie dogs will often fall silent. | 0:35:29 | 0:35:35 | |
To call out now could prove fatal. | 0:35:35 | 0:35:38 | |
If a pup does indeed learn from its parents, | 0:35:43 | 0:35:45 | |
then knowing when to hold fire and retreat | 0:35:45 | 0:35:48 | |
could be one of the most valuable lessons of its life... | 0:35:48 | 0:35:51 | |
SQUEAKING | 0:36:00 | 0:36:03 | |
..or the end of its life, if it gets it wrong. | 0:36:09 | 0:36:14 | |
When one of your neighbours is killed, | 0:36:21 | 0:36:23 | |
it wouldn't seem unreasonable to expect a moment of quiet reflection. | 0:36:23 | 0:36:29 | |
But once the swift fox is out of sight, | 0:36:34 | 0:36:38 | |
something quite extraordinary happens in prairie dog town. | 0:36:38 | 0:36:43 | |
LOUD BARKING | 0:36:43 | 0:36:46 | |
It looks like the prairie dogs are experiencing a moment of madness. | 0:36:46 | 0:36:50 | |
This acrobatic display is in fact the jump-yip call. | 0:36:52 | 0:36:58 | |
It could be an all-clear signal when a threat or danger has passed. | 0:36:58 | 0:37:05 | |
Whatever the meaning, it's certainly infectious. | 0:37:05 | 0:37:08 | |
In late summer, the rains bring a flush of colour to the prairies. | 0:37:23 | 0:37:28 | |
For the prairie dogs, it's been a pretty boring diet until now, | 0:38:11 | 0:38:15 | |
but sunflowers will provide a tasty treat. | 0:38:15 | 0:38:20 | |
Each sunflower seed has twice the number of calories | 0:38:21 | 0:38:24 | |
as a blade of grass, so they're vital to the prairie dog pups | 0:38:24 | 0:38:28 | |
as they fatten up for the winter. | 0:38:28 | 0:38:30 | |
Time to set up their final exams | 0:38:36 | 0:38:38 | |
and see how their vocabulary is shaping up. | 0:38:38 | 0:38:41 | |
Are they still babbling like babies, or talking like adults? | 0:38:41 | 0:38:46 | |
Out come the usual suspects. | 0:38:46 | 0:38:49 | |
SQUEAKING | 0:39:11 | 0:39:13 | |
That's wonderful. We've got a really distinct coyote call. | 0:39:17 | 0:39:21 | |
There's another one calling from across the colony. | 0:39:21 | 0:39:25 | |
OK, Bill, let's do the badger. | 0:39:46 | 0:39:48 | |
They're standing up where they're foraging and looking | 0:40:02 | 0:40:05 | |
to see where the badger is going, and that's different from the coyote. | 0:40:05 | 0:40:09 | |
For the coyote, they run to the lip of their burrow and stand there. | 0:40:09 | 0:40:13 | |
For the badger, they are a little bit more reluctant to | 0:40:13 | 0:40:16 | |
run to their burrow until they see which burrow the badger is going to. | 0:40:16 | 0:40:20 | |
The signs are all good. | 0:40:22 | 0:40:23 | |
They are at least reacting in the right way to the fake predators. | 0:40:23 | 0:40:28 | |
The pups seem to be in fine voice. | 0:40:30 | 0:40:34 | |
But have they learnt the proper words from the adults? | 0:40:34 | 0:40:38 | |
To find out, they must compare them with pup calls | 0:40:40 | 0:40:43 | |
from earlier in the year to see if they've changed. | 0:40:43 | 0:40:47 | |
So let's just play the call that we got from the pups for the badger... | 0:40:47 | 0:40:51 | |
-OK. -..two months ago versus the calls that we got from them today. | 0:40:51 | 0:40:56 | |
Let's see if we can hear it. | 0:40:56 | 0:40:58 | |
SQUEAKING | 0:40:58 | 0:41:00 | |
Yeah, again, very non-structured calls early in the season | 0:41:07 | 0:41:11 | |
and highly structured calls later in the season. | 0:41:11 | 0:41:13 | |
Looking at this spectrogram, you really see the difference. | 0:41:13 | 0:41:17 | |
-You do. -Very detailed in the adult call. | 0:41:17 | 0:41:19 | |
What we saw at the beginning of the season | 0:41:19 | 0:41:21 | |
was that the pups gave very non-specific, generic calls, | 0:41:21 | 0:41:25 | |
and now these same pups are giving highly structured calls. | 0:41:25 | 0:41:29 | |
So, clearly, the pups have learned in just two months | 0:41:29 | 0:41:33 | |
how to adapt their calls so that they sound the same as the adult calls. | 0:41:33 | 0:41:37 | |
Let's see the same thing with the coyote. | 0:41:37 | 0:41:39 | |
Yeah, let's do that for the coyote. | 0:41:39 | 0:41:41 | |
Here we go. | 0:41:41 | 0:41:44 | |
Wow. | 0:41:44 | 0:41:46 | |
Again non-specific, just squeaks versus a lot of acoustic structure. | 0:41:46 | 0:41:52 | |
That's really very impressive. | 0:41:52 | 0:41:54 | |
-The calls are longer duration, as well. -Yeah. | 0:41:54 | 0:41:57 | |
Probably because they contain a lot more information in them. | 0:41:57 | 0:42:01 | |
I think this shows that learning is actually going on, | 0:42:03 | 0:42:06 | |
because clearly we went from very generic, | 0:42:06 | 0:42:09 | |
non-specific calls to very specific calls in just a short period of time. | 0:42:09 | 0:42:15 | |
And I don't think that there's another explanation | 0:42:15 | 0:42:19 | |
other than learning. | 0:42:19 | 0:42:20 | |
It is possible that there is a genetic programme | 0:42:20 | 0:42:23 | |
that causes the maturation of the vocal tract, | 0:42:23 | 0:42:26 | |
but I think that's relatively unlikely | 0:42:26 | 0:42:28 | |
compared to the possibility of learning. | 0:42:28 | 0:42:31 | |
It's a eureka moment for Con and for the prairie dogs. | 0:42:32 | 0:42:39 | |
They have passed all the language tests with flying colours. | 0:42:39 | 0:42:42 | |
For Con, these findings are more than just about science. | 0:42:44 | 0:42:48 | |
He hopes they'll make people look differently at prairie dogs | 0:42:48 | 0:42:52 | |
and not see them as just vermin. | 0:42:52 | 0:42:54 | |
Prairie dogs have a very complicated social system. | 0:42:54 | 0:42:57 | |
They have to have some sense of their relationships between each other | 0:42:57 | 0:43:02 | |
in order to maintain this social system. | 0:43:02 | 0:43:05 | |
They also have this complex language, | 0:43:05 | 0:43:07 | |
and they have to have some kind of intellectual structure | 0:43:07 | 0:43:11 | |
in order to be able to understand this language, as well. | 0:43:11 | 0:43:14 | |
So the two things combined says to us that prairie dogs | 0:43:14 | 0:43:18 | |
are really very complex animals. | 0:43:18 | 0:43:20 | |
After 30 years of research, Con has all the information he needs | 0:43:20 | 0:43:25 | |
to help prove his bold claim that prairie dogs | 0:43:25 | 0:43:28 | |
have the most sophisticated language in the animal world. | 0:43:28 | 0:43:33 | |
At last the prairie dogs can step from the shadow of the bison | 0:43:36 | 0:43:40 | |
and take centre stage as the true hero of the prairies. | 0:43:40 | 0:43:44 | |
And they have one man to thank for that. | 0:43:44 | 0:43:47 |