Browse content similar to Episode 3. Check below for episodes and series from the same categories and more!
Line | From | To | |
---|---|---|---|
However well we think we know our planet, the natural world | 0:00:08 | 0:00:12 | |
still has the power to surprise us, to shock us, sometimes even to | 0:00:12 | 0:00:18 | |
scare us with its extraordinary events and bizarre behaviour. | 0:00:18 | 0:00:23 | |
Given modern technology, nature's weirdest phenomena are frequently | 0:00:23 | 0:00:28 | |
caught on camera wherever and whenever they occur, and this means | 0:00:28 | 0:00:33 | |
that we can bring you the strangest stories our world has to offer. | 0:00:33 | 0:00:37 | |
From animals that keel over at the first sign of trouble, | 0:00:39 | 0:00:45 | |
to bears grabbing a takeaway. | 0:00:45 | 0:00:47 | |
And a pigeon-snatching predator from the deep. | 0:00:49 | 0:00:52 | |
With the help of eye-witnesses, experts and scientists, | 0:00:54 | 0:00:58 | |
we're going to try and explain what on earth is going on. | 0:00:58 | 0:01:02 | |
The natural world can astound us with intriguing behaviour and | 0:01:23 | 0:01:27 | |
extraordinary designs, and in these weird events, | 0:01:27 | 0:01:31 | |
nature seems to have torn up the blueprint altogether | 0:01:31 | 0:01:35 | |
and come up with something truly bizarre. | 0:01:35 | 0:01:39 | |
Welcome to the world of nature's weirdest designs. | 0:01:39 | 0:01:43 | |
We're starting with the animals whose design appears to have | 0:01:43 | 0:01:46 | |
gone wrong right at the drawing board. | 0:01:46 | 0:01:49 | |
This is the stuff of myths and legends, | 0:01:50 | 0:01:53 | |
creatures so strange, they have to be seen to be believed. | 0:01:53 | 0:01:57 | |
From a lobster that looks half-baked, to a snake in two minds. | 0:01:57 | 0:02:03 | |
But first, we're off to the USA to meet some animals that may | 0:02:04 | 0:02:09 | |
look normal, yet something is making them act very strangely indeed. | 0:02:09 | 0:02:15 | |
Healthy looking goats that appear to drop down dead at the drop of a hat. | 0:02:17 | 0:02:21 | |
Oh, no, I've killed my wife's goat! | 0:02:23 | 0:02:25 | |
When the farmer Gene McNutt and his family moved to the peace | 0:02:29 | 0:02:32 | |
and quiet of Tennessee, they had no idea of the strange events | 0:02:32 | 0:02:37 | |
that would befall them. | 0:02:37 | 0:02:39 | |
We moved here to Tennessee in 1988. | 0:02:39 | 0:02:44 | |
My wife immediately wanted a new goat. | 0:02:44 | 0:02:48 | |
One of our neighbours was kind enough to bring her home | 0:02:49 | 0:02:52 | |
a six or eight week old goat and that goat became the pet of the house. | 0:02:52 | 0:02:57 | |
The McNutts adored their pet goat, | 0:02:57 | 0:03:01 | |
but then Gene had a very unnerving experience. | 0:03:01 | 0:03:05 | |
One day, the goat falls down and looks like he's gone stiff with | 0:03:06 | 0:03:13 | |
rigor mortis and I say, "Oh, no, I killed my wife's goat." | 0:03:13 | 0:03:18 | |
"What have I done? Has it had a heart attack?" | 0:03:20 | 0:03:22 | |
I'm standing there just in total amazement, | 0:03:22 | 0:03:26 | |
trying to figure out what am I going to do. | 0:03:26 | 0:03:28 | |
Had Gene suddenly developed the power to kill goats | 0:03:28 | 0:03:32 | |
just by looking at them? | 0:03:32 | 0:03:33 | |
Well, no. | 0:03:37 | 0:03:39 | |
Suddenly the goat wriggles a little bit, | 0:03:39 | 0:03:42 | |
gets up and walks off as if nothing had happened. | 0:03:42 | 0:03:45 | |
I decided to say nothing to my wife about it | 0:03:45 | 0:03:49 | |
since the goats appeared to be OK. | 0:03:49 | 0:03:51 | |
I asked the neighbour about the goat | 0:03:51 | 0:03:55 | |
because it was something that had never happened before, | 0:03:55 | 0:03:58 | |
and he says, "Oh," he says, "Those are just old nervous goats." | 0:03:58 | 0:04:02 | |
He says, "They're from around here." | 0:04:02 | 0:04:05 | |
Gene's experience wasn't a one-off. | 0:04:05 | 0:04:08 | |
These goats of the Deep South have a tendency to head south, | 0:04:11 | 0:04:15 | |
and then rise again unharmed. | 0:04:15 | 0:04:18 | |
So what's causing the goats of Tennessee to keel over, | 0:04:20 | 0:04:23 | |
only to be resurrected shortly afterwards? | 0:04:23 | 0:04:26 | |
Well, there could be a clue in what triggers their downfall. | 0:04:29 | 0:04:33 | |
Almost any noise that is unusual will cause the goat to stiffen | 0:04:34 | 0:04:41 | |
or fall over and it'll stay in a fallen position for 30, | 0:04:41 | 0:04:47 | |
45 seconds and sometimes even up to a couple of minutes. | 0:04:47 | 0:04:50 | |
Shirley's down. | 0:04:50 | 0:04:53 | |
Sometimes just a quick movement by a person will cause the goat | 0:04:53 | 0:04:56 | |
to fall over. | 0:04:56 | 0:04:58 | |
Almost every morning when I feed the goats, | 0:04:58 | 0:05:01 | |
at least one of them will fall out | 0:05:01 | 0:05:03 | |
and all I'm doing is pouring feed out of the bucket. | 0:05:03 | 0:05:06 | |
So, stress or excitement seems to be a factor in falling over. | 0:05:08 | 0:05:13 | |
When threatened or surprised, | 0:05:14 | 0:05:16 | |
many animals have a fight or flight response. | 0:05:16 | 0:05:20 | |
Their muscles suddenly tense up, as they prepare to run away or attack. | 0:05:20 | 0:05:24 | |
Freezing for a moment is part of a vital survival strategy | 0:05:26 | 0:05:30 | |
for some wild animals, but it seems that these goats in Tennessee | 0:05:30 | 0:05:35 | |
have a glitch in their system. | 0:05:35 | 0:05:38 | |
Instead of freezing for just a split second, they completely seize up. | 0:05:38 | 0:05:42 | |
This unusual reaction is unique to a particular breed called | 0:05:43 | 0:05:47 | |
fainting goats, although they don't actually lose consciousness. | 0:05:47 | 0:05:52 | |
So what makes this happen? | 0:05:52 | 0:05:54 | |
Well, it's all down to a genetic disorder called myotonia congenita, | 0:05:56 | 0:06:01 | |
and it affects how their muscles work. | 0:06:01 | 0:06:04 | |
The muscle fibres contract as normal, but then they don't relax | 0:06:04 | 0:06:08 | |
afterwards as they should, so these goats become temporarily paralysed. | 0:06:08 | 0:06:12 | |
It's a condition that they learn to live with. | 0:06:13 | 0:06:16 | |
When they're young, a lot of things will make them faint, | 0:06:16 | 0:06:21 | |
but as they get older, even though they'll stiffen up, | 0:06:21 | 0:06:23 | |
most of the time they'll find something to lean on | 0:06:23 | 0:06:27 | |
or they'll get their balance because they know that if they're off balance | 0:06:27 | 0:06:32 | |
then they'll indeed fall over. | 0:06:32 | 0:06:34 | |
These domesticated goats don't seem to suffer any ill effects, | 0:06:36 | 0:06:39 | |
but clearly this could be a problem in the wild. | 0:06:39 | 0:06:42 | |
Wild goats are renowned for their agility. | 0:06:44 | 0:06:46 | |
If they fell over whenever they were startled or a predator approached, | 0:06:50 | 0:06:54 | |
this genetic flaw would prove fatal. | 0:06:54 | 0:06:57 | |
So why did this weird phenomenon turn up in the goats of Tennessee? | 0:07:13 | 0:07:17 | |
There's a local legend to explain their genetic quirk. | 0:07:19 | 0:07:22 | |
About 1885, a man came to a little community called | 0:07:25 | 0:07:30 | |
Caney Springs here in Marshall County, Tennessee. | 0:07:30 | 0:07:34 | |
With him he brought three or four little goats. | 0:07:34 | 0:07:37 | |
The unique thing about these three little goats, | 0:07:37 | 0:07:39 | |
is that when they're startled, they fall over. | 0:07:39 | 0:07:44 | |
All fainting goats alive today are descended from that one small | 0:07:47 | 0:07:51 | |
original flock and it appears that inbreeding has led to some | 0:07:51 | 0:07:56 | |
genetical mis-wiring and their natural defence system | 0:07:56 | 0:07:59 | |
has gone awry. | 0:07:59 | 0:08:01 | |
Now, of course, in the wild, this behaviour would be treacherous. | 0:08:01 | 0:08:04 | |
It doesn't seem to be too much of a problem in a domestic herd, | 0:08:04 | 0:08:07 | |
but then having said that, to us, it seems very strange indeed. | 0:08:07 | 0:08:11 | |
From fainting goats that foxed many farmers, | 0:08:13 | 0:08:16 | |
we move on to sea creatures which are freaking out fishermen, | 0:08:16 | 0:08:20 | |
with a design flaw that's clear for all to see. | 0:08:20 | 0:08:24 | |
We head to the Atlantic Coast of North America, where a fisherman | 0:08:27 | 0:08:32 | |
landed a lobster that will have you adjusting your TV sets. | 0:08:32 | 0:08:36 | |
I just jumped aboard the vessel. | 0:08:36 | 0:08:40 | |
Just caught this blue Maine lobster. | 0:08:40 | 0:08:45 | |
An astonishing bright blue lobster. | 0:08:45 | 0:08:48 | |
A vivid contrast to its normally-coloured companion, | 0:08:48 | 0:08:52 | |
and if one electric blue lobster wasn't shocking enough, | 0:08:52 | 0:08:56 | |
another turned up, but it didn't stop there. | 0:08:56 | 0:09:00 | |
Ah, we got ourselves another crazy lobster down here. | 0:09:01 | 0:09:04 | |
Pretty funky, pretty cool. | 0:09:04 | 0:09:06 | |
Another was brilliant orange, the colour that lobsters go | 0:09:08 | 0:09:13 | |
when they're cooked, and yet this one was very much alive. | 0:09:13 | 0:09:16 | |
And just when they thought they'd seen it all, up from the depths came | 0:09:17 | 0:09:21 | |
this, an impossible looking lobster with a jaw dropping colour split. | 0:09:21 | 0:09:28 | |
So, what on earth can explain these colourful crustaceans? | 0:09:31 | 0:09:35 | |
To find out, we need to delve deeper and take a closer look | 0:09:36 | 0:09:39 | |
at the colouring of normal-looking lobsters. | 0:09:39 | 0:09:42 | |
This is what an adult lobster looks like in the wild | 0:09:44 | 0:09:48 | |
and they get their colour as they grow, from the food that they eat. | 0:09:48 | 0:09:52 | |
They feed on a diet of crustaceans, shellfish and zooplankton | 0:09:52 | 0:09:56 | |
and many of these organisms have a red pigment inside them | 0:09:56 | 0:09:59 | |
called astaxanthin. | 0:09:59 | 0:10:01 | |
It's the same pigment that makes salmon pink. | 0:10:01 | 0:10:05 | |
And it even gives flamingos their trademark colour. | 0:10:05 | 0:10:08 | |
So why don't lobsters turn pink if they're also eating astaxanthin? | 0:10:09 | 0:10:14 | |
When the red pigment is absorbed into the lobster's body, | 0:10:16 | 0:10:18 | |
it's laid down in its muscles and into a membrane that | 0:10:18 | 0:10:22 | |
lies beneath the shell. | 0:10:22 | 0:10:23 | |
But over a period of time, it migrates into the shell | 0:10:23 | 0:10:27 | |
where it's transformed by a protein into a blue pigment. | 0:10:27 | 0:10:31 | |
A little later, it migrates again right up to the | 0:10:31 | 0:10:35 | |
surface of the shell where it meets another protein which | 0:10:35 | 0:10:38 | |
transforms it for a second time into a yellow pigment like this. | 0:10:38 | 0:10:43 | |
Now, when they are all overlaid, we get to see what we see | 0:10:43 | 0:10:47 | |
when we look at the lobster, this dirty brown-y green colour. | 0:10:47 | 0:10:51 | |
This layering of colour provides good | 0:10:51 | 0:10:54 | |
camouflage down on the sea bed where lobsters spend most of their lives. | 0:10:54 | 0:10:58 | |
It keeps the young safely hidden and allows adults to stalk their prey. | 0:10:59 | 0:11:03 | |
Understanding the mix of colour layers in normal lobsters | 0:11:05 | 0:11:08 | |
means we can work out how this design can go wrong. | 0:11:08 | 0:11:13 | |
What happens if we remove one of those pigment layers, | 0:11:13 | 0:11:16 | |
say the blue one? | 0:11:16 | 0:11:17 | |
Well, then we're left with this, an orange-looking lobster. | 0:11:17 | 0:11:21 | |
But why would a lobster be missing a blue layer? | 0:11:22 | 0:11:25 | |
Crustacean expert Dr Grant Stentaford | 0:11:27 | 0:11:30 | |
knows the answer. | 0:11:30 | 0:11:31 | |
The orange lobsters which are abnormal, are actually missing | 0:11:31 | 0:11:34 | |
a very important protein, a blue protein which is | 0:11:34 | 0:11:37 | |
normally in the shell, and that's not produced because of a genetic defect. | 0:11:37 | 0:11:40 | |
So, it's a genetic design flaw that makes some lobsters bright orange. | 0:11:40 | 0:11:46 | |
So could the same be true for blue lobsters? | 0:11:46 | 0:11:49 | |
Well, people may think it's | 0:11:49 | 0:11:51 | |
because the lobster is missing the red or the yellow pigment | 0:11:51 | 0:11:54 | |
from the shell, but actually, it's more likely to be due to an | 0:11:54 | 0:11:57 | |
over-expression of the blue protein, or possibly the fact that the lobster | 0:11:57 | 0:12:01 | |
is not absorbing those red pigments from the diet that it's eating. | 0:12:01 | 0:12:05 | |
So the strange looking lobsters of America's East Coast | 0:12:06 | 0:12:09 | |
are in fact extremely rare colour mutants. | 0:12:09 | 0:12:13 | |
But what about that extraordinary two-tone lobster? | 0:12:14 | 0:12:18 | |
What's going on there? | 0:12:18 | 0:12:19 | |
Just before Halloween in 2012, in the waters | 0:12:21 | 0:12:25 | |
off of Salem, Massachusetts, this creepy crustacean was hauled in. | 0:12:25 | 0:12:31 | |
It's mutant orange on one half of its body, | 0:12:32 | 0:12:35 | |
but normal coloured on the other. | 0:12:35 | 0:12:37 | |
The men who caught it, Dana and Ryan Duhaime, | 0:12:40 | 0:12:43 | |
had never seen anything like it before. | 0:12:43 | 0:12:45 | |
Right away when it came on deck I said, "What is this?" | 0:12:45 | 0:12:48 | |
I couldn't believe we had a lobster that was orange on one side | 0:12:48 | 0:12:51 | |
and black on the other and straight, straight, right down the middle. | 0:12:51 | 0:12:54 | |
We couldn't wait to take it in to show these guys on the dock, | 0:12:54 | 0:12:57 | |
you know, what do you think of this? | 0:12:57 | 0:12:59 | |
Unlike us, each side of a lobster's body | 0:13:01 | 0:13:04 | |
develops completely independently. | 0:13:04 | 0:13:06 | |
To explain this, we have to go right back to the drawing board. | 0:13:08 | 0:13:11 | |
Let's look at the beginning of a lobster's life. | 0:13:11 | 0:13:14 | |
Once the sperm has fertilised the egg, | 0:13:16 | 0:13:19 | |
it grows and splits into two cells. | 0:13:19 | 0:13:21 | |
And this is an absolutely critical time for the lobster, | 0:13:23 | 0:13:28 | |
because this initial spit will define how each side of its body | 0:13:28 | 0:13:32 | |
develops and remains for the rest of its life. | 0:13:32 | 0:13:35 | |
Now very occasionally, as it makes this initial split, | 0:13:35 | 0:13:39 | |
there's a genetic mutation, say in the form of the genes | 0:13:39 | 0:13:43 | |
that code for colour. | 0:13:43 | 0:13:45 | |
So, if there's an abnormality on one side, as each of these cells | 0:13:45 | 0:13:49 | |
is replicated and the lobster grows, then that genetic | 0:13:49 | 0:13:53 | |
abnormality will be replicated too. | 0:13:53 | 0:13:56 | |
What we end up with is this, | 0:13:56 | 0:13:58 | |
a lobster split directly in two with a weird colour mutation on one | 0:13:58 | 0:14:03 | |
side and it's all down to that initial cell split. | 0:14:03 | 0:14:08 | |
And that explains what happened with our Halloween lobster. | 0:14:08 | 0:14:12 | |
But, incredibly, it's not unique. | 0:14:12 | 0:14:15 | |
This one from Newfoundland has an even stranger design flaw | 0:14:15 | 0:14:19 | |
which can also be traced back to the original cell split. | 0:14:19 | 0:14:24 | |
As well as turning black and blue, | 0:14:24 | 0:14:26 | |
it has a mutation in its sex chromosomes. | 0:14:26 | 0:14:29 | |
And this means that we end up with a lobster which has female sex | 0:14:29 | 0:14:35 | |
organs on one half of its body and male sex organs on the other half. | 0:14:35 | 0:14:41 | |
Almost all of the two-tone lobsters ever recorded have been | 0:14:41 | 0:14:45 | |
different sexes on each side. | 0:14:45 | 0:14:47 | |
This rare half-and-half layout is found in other species, | 0:14:49 | 0:14:53 | |
again where each side of the body develops independently | 0:14:53 | 0:14:57 | |
and where the males and the female look different. | 0:14:57 | 0:15:00 | |
We'll see it in butterflies and other insects. | 0:15:03 | 0:15:06 | |
In those cases, again, the sexes are different on both | 0:15:06 | 0:15:09 | |
sides of the animal, and we often see wing differences | 0:15:09 | 0:15:12 | |
so they can be different shapes and even different colours. | 0:15:12 | 0:15:15 | |
Two-tone lobsters have been surprising fishermen for a | 0:15:18 | 0:15:21 | |
while now, but scientists have only recently unravelled | 0:15:21 | 0:15:25 | |
the colourful tales that explain their appearance. | 0:15:25 | 0:15:28 | |
And with most lobster populations off the Atlantic Coast increasing, | 0:15:29 | 0:15:33 | |
we'll no doubt see more of these weird and wonderful colour mutants. | 0:15:33 | 0:15:38 | |
So, colourful crustacean conundrum solved. | 0:15:41 | 0:15:45 | |
Now to meet a true creature of legend, face to face. | 0:15:45 | 0:15:48 | |
On September 17, 2013, snake breeder Ross Gregersen | 0:15:51 | 0:15:55 | |
was checking the Mojave python eggs he was incubating. | 0:15:55 | 0:15:59 | |
So here's our last clutch of the year, here's the first Mojave. | 0:15:59 | 0:16:03 | |
-Ross made a shocking discovery. -Here's our second one. | 0:16:04 | 0:16:08 | |
When I first saw him, I thought he's either dead | 0:16:09 | 0:16:12 | |
or he's kinked or something cos his neck's pretty goofy looking. | 0:16:12 | 0:16:15 | |
I poked him and he shifted and I saw this | 0:16:15 | 0:16:19 | |
and realised that I have a two headed Mojave. | 0:16:19 | 0:16:23 | |
Amazingly, both heads of this snake are alive. | 0:16:23 | 0:16:27 | |
And it's not a one-off. | 0:16:30 | 0:16:33 | |
For snake enthusiasts, two-headed animals are highly prized, | 0:16:33 | 0:16:38 | |
especially when the split is perfect and symmetrical like this one. | 0:16:38 | 0:16:42 | |
Ben Siegel owns a reptile shop | 0:16:45 | 0:16:48 | |
and his striking looking snake has become world famous. | 0:16:48 | 0:16:52 | |
This is Medusa, she's our two-headed albino Honduran milk snake. | 0:16:53 | 0:16:57 | |
She was born a couple of years ago and she's the mascot at our store. | 0:16:58 | 0:17:04 | |
I've been keeping snakes since I was six, so about 34 years, | 0:17:04 | 0:17:09 | |
and I've never quite seen one like her before. | 0:17:09 | 0:17:11 | |
We love her, she's really special to us. | 0:17:11 | 0:17:14 | |
She's perfect other than... she's more than perfect. | 0:17:14 | 0:17:18 | |
So how do snakes end up with two heads? | 0:17:20 | 0:17:24 | |
And what kind of double life do they have to lead? | 0:17:24 | 0:17:27 | |
A snake with two heads is the result of incomplete twinning. | 0:17:29 | 0:17:32 | |
It happens when the snake embryo is developing, | 0:17:34 | 0:17:37 | |
either in the egg or the womb. | 0:17:37 | 0:17:39 | |
The same thing can happen with human embryos | 0:17:40 | 0:17:43 | |
if they split into identical twins. | 0:17:43 | 0:17:45 | |
A glitch in the separation process means it grinds to a halt. | 0:17:46 | 0:17:51 | |
In this case the result is conjoined snake twins each with a head. | 0:17:51 | 0:17:56 | |
But as this X-ray shows, the head joins a single spine | 0:17:57 | 0:18:01 | |
and they share the rest of their body. | 0:18:01 | 0:18:04 | |
Not surprisingly, such an unusual design | 0:18:04 | 0:18:07 | |
sets the snake several challenges. | 0:18:07 | 0:18:10 | |
Gordon Burghardt at the University of Tennessee | 0:18:14 | 0:18:17 | |
has kept and studied two-headed snakes. | 0:18:17 | 0:18:19 | |
We had one animal, | 0:18:20 | 0:18:22 | |
that black rat snake that lived for about 20 years and we recorded | 0:18:22 | 0:18:28 | |
lots of its behaviour, | 0:18:28 | 0:18:30 | |
but in the wild, they have, I think, great difficulty. | 0:18:30 | 0:18:33 | |
For a start, how does a two-headed snake decide which way to go? | 0:18:35 | 0:18:39 | |
Well, let's look at how normal snakes move | 0:18:42 | 0:18:44 | |
through the world in their natural environment. | 0:18:44 | 0:18:48 | |
With no limbs, they propel themselves along | 0:18:48 | 0:18:51 | |
with a slithering motion. | 0:18:51 | 0:18:53 | |
Snakes can only travel quickly for short bursts, | 0:18:53 | 0:18:57 | |
but if a predator appears, they need to make a split second decision | 0:18:57 | 0:19:01 | |
to find cover. | 0:19:01 | 0:19:03 | |
This is a problem for a two-headed snake | 0:19:04 | 0:19:08 | |
because each head has an independently thinking brain, | 0:19:08 | 0:19:12 | |
so it's always in two minds about which way to go. | 0:19:12 | 0:19:16 | |
But getting around isn't their only problem. | 0:19:18 | 0:19:21 | |
What happens when they want to eat? | 0:19:21 | 0:19:25 | |
When we feed her, we take and put her in an empty, clean, open tub, | 0:19:25 | 0:19:28 | |
and one of our employees actually takes two food items at the same time | 0:19:28 | 0:19:33 | |
and tries to lure one head to one side and one head to the other side, | 0:19:33 | 0:19:36 | |
trying to introduce the food at exactly the same time, | 0:19:36 | 0:19:39 | |
so they both grab it. | 0:19:39 | 0:19:40 | |
Often at times, we'll actually, when one head swallows it down quickly, | 0:19:40 | 0:19:44 | |
we'll give it another food item so that ideally you want them to both | 0:19:44 | 0:19:47 | |
finish at the same time. | 0:19:47 | 0:19:48 | |
Then after we feed her, we have to take her | 0:19:48 | 0:19:50 | |
and wash her heads off with water and soap, | 0:19:50 | 0:19:52 | |
so that she doesn't smell like her food because then both heads | 0:19:52 | 0:19:56 | |
will try to attack each other just from the smell of the food | 0:19:56 | 0:19:58 | |
on the head. | 0:19:58 | 0:20:00 | |
This may sound bizarre, but smell is a critical sense for snakes. | 0:20:01 | 0:20:07 | |
Although some have good eyesight when it comes to | 0:20:07 | 0:20:09 | |
finding their prey, most snakes rely on smell more than vision. | 0:20:09 | 0:20:14 | |
Their tongues play a vital part in smelling. | 0:20:16 | 0:20:19 | |
They waft odour molecules into a structure called | 0:20:21 | 0:20:24 | |
the Jacobson's organ, which detects the smell. | 0:20:24 | 0:20:30 | |
When it comes to the smell of food, | 0:20:35 | 0:20:37 | |
they follow their tongues to the meal. | 0:20:37 | 0:20:39 | |
For a two-headed snake, a meal could be confused with | 0:20:41 | 0:20:44 | |
the smell of leftovers on its other half. | 0:20:44 | 0:20:47 | |
Evidence surely that two heads aren't always better than one. | 0:20:47 | 0:20:51 | |
Fortunately, Medusa is well cared for and this helps reduce | 0:20:53 | 0:20:57 | |
the chance of a mishap. | 0:20:57 | 0:20:59 | |
But in some cases, this competition for food may be more | 0:21:00 | 0:21:03 | |
deep-seated than we imagine, | 0:21:03 | 0:21:06 | |
as Gordon discovered with one of his two-headed snakes. | 0:21:06 | 0:21:10 | |
From all the records we had heard about, | 0:21:10 | 0:21:12 | |
the two heads, the animals have necks and they swallow the prey | 0:21:12 | 0:21:15 | |
and they go to the common stomach. | 0:21:15 | 0:21:17 | |
So this was what we had thought was going on, | 0:21:17 | 0:21:20 | |
until we did some x-rays of the animal actually feeding | 0:21:20 | 0:21:23 | |
and what we found is that the animal actually had two stomachs. | 0:21:23 | 0:21:28 | |
So their motivational systems were somewhat independent. | 0:21:28 | 0:21:32 | |
These were individual animals in a sense that they did co-operate | 0:21:32 | 0:21:35 | |
and learn to do maybe some things a little bit better together, | 0:21:35 | 0:21:39 | |
but, basically, they were two minds in the same body. | 0:21:39 | 0:21:42 | |
So, two-headed snakes are rare enough in captivity. | 0:21:45 | 0:21:48 | |
Just imagine seeing one in the wild. | 0:21:48 | 0:21:52 | |
Well, believe it or not, that's what happened fairly recently | 0:21:52 | 0:21:55 | |
when a group of hikers were out in the Yorkshire hills. | 0:21:55 | 0:21:58 | |
They spotted one of these, an adder, Britain's only venomous | 0:21:58 | 0:22:01 | |
species of snake, and snapped a few photos before it slithered away. | 0:22:01 | 0:22:05 | |
It was only when they looked at them more closely afterwards, | 0:22:05 | 0:22:08 | |
they recognised that the snake actually had two heads. | 0:22:08 | 0:22:13 | |
An amazing rarity, a UK first and experts think that the animal | 0:22:13 | 0:22:18 | |
is already a few months, perhaps even a year old, | 0:22:18 | 0:22:21 | |
so it's doing a good job of surviving in the wild. | 0:22:21 | 0:22:25 | |
I'm absolutely enthralled by the thought that this snake | 0:22:25 | 0:22:28 | |
might still be out there somewhere. | 0:22:28 | 0:22:31 | |
All of these stories make us stop | 0:22:33 | 0:22:35 | |
and think twice about what happens when nature's blueprints go wrong. | 0:22:35 | 0:22:39 | |
Whether it's goats overcome by the slightest excitement, | 0:22:41 | 0:22:46 | |
mutant lobsters equally in touch with their masculine | 0:22:46 | 0:22:50 | |
and feminine sides, or snakes who are in two minds about life. | 0:22:50 | 0:22:57 | |
These wonderful, exceptional animals remind us | 0:22:57 | 0:23:00 | |
that, incredibly, most of the time, nature gets it completely right. | 0:23:00 | 0:23:04 | |
Next, we meet animals that are pushing the limits of what | 0:23:07 | 0:23:10 | |
they're designed to do, deploying shocking tactics to secure a meal. | 0:23:10 | 0:23:17 | |
There's a fish taking a leap into the unknown, | 0:23:17 | 0:23:20 | |
and a bird using brain power to expand its menu. | 0:23:20 | 0:23:24 | |
But first, we're heading to the State of Colorado to investigate | 0:23:24 | 0:23:28 | |
an alarming animal crime wave. | 0:23:28 | 0:23:30 | |
It's the early hours of July 31st, 2013 and a shifty individual | 0:23:32 | 0:23:39 | |
is targeting a one-tonne waste bin at the back of this restaurant. | 0:23:39 | 0:23:43 | |
This isn't so much dumpster diving as dumpster driving. | 0:23:49 | 0:23:55 | |
The next night, he comes back and steals another dumpster. | 0:23:58 | 0:24:01 | |
This black bear is a repeat offender. | 0:24:04 | 0:24:06 | |
On another night in July, a different shady character is caught | 0:24:09 | 0:24:13 | |
breaking into this chocolate shop in the town of Estes Park. | 0:24:13 | 0:24:17 | |
This is a high calorie heist. | 0:24:21 | 0:24:24 | |
So, what's driving these bears to commit a summer crime wave | 0:24:29 | 0:24:32 | |
in Colorado? | 0:24:32 | 0:24:34 | |
Well, when it comes to diet, | 0:24:36 | 0:24:38 | |
black bears eat pretty much what we eat. | 0:24:38 | 0:24:41 | |
Fruit, vegetables, meat and fish, | 0:24:41 | 0:24:44 | |
so our food makes a good meal for them too. | 0:24:44 | 0:24:47 | |
This restaurant robbery took place in Colorado Springs. | 0:24:51 | 0:24:55 | |
It's nestled at the base of the Rocky Mountains, | 0:24:55 | 0:24:57 | |
an area with a huge population of black bears. | 0:24:57 | 0:25:01 | |
But this is the scene of the crime, a full two miles into the city. | 0:25:03 | 0:25:09 | |
The bear went for the bins around the back, so what lured him | 0:25:09 | 0:25:15 | |
so far into town? | 0:25:15 | 0:25:17 | |
Well, he was almost certainly led by his nose. | 0:25:18 | 0:25:21 | |
Bears have a sense of smell that's even more powerful than bloodhounds. | 0:25:22 | 0:25:27 | |
Our bear probably smelled the city leftovers from miles away. | 0:25:27 | 0:25:31 | |
In fact, he might have visited several | 0:25:32 | 0:25:35 | |
restaurants before being caught on camera at Edelweiss. | 0:25:35 | 0:25:38 | |
So now we know how the bear targeted the restaurant. | 0:25:41 | 0:25:45 | |
But how did he manage to move the heavy dumpster? | 0:25:46 | 0:25:49 | |
Wildlife cameraman Jeff Turner, | 0:25:51 | 0:25:54 | |
who's filmed bears for decades, has some ideas. | 0:25:54 | 0:25:57 | |
The bear's able to do what he does with that dumpster | 0:25:57 | 0:26:00 | |
for three basic reasons. | 0:26:00 | 0:26:01 | |
The first reason is the way that his feet are built. | 0:26:01 | 0:26:04 | |
His feet are built much like a human's or a great ape. | 0:26:04 | 0:26:08 | |
He can actually stand on the flat of his feet which allows him to | 0:26:08 | 0:26:12 | |
stand up quite comfortably in order to reach the top of the dumpster. | 0:26:12 | 0:26:15 | |
And then he's got very dexterous paws like his claws | 0:26:15 | 0:26:19 | |
and individual use of those digits and his paws allow him | 0:26:19 | 0:26:23 | |
to actually grip things. | 0:26:23 | 0:26:25 | |
The edge of that dumpster, he can get a good grip on it. | 0:26:25 | 0:26:28 | |
And then the third element that allows him | 0:26:28 | 0:26:30 | |
to move it is his strength. They're very, very strong animals, | 0:26:30 | 0:26:33 | |
very, very powerful, so it's nothing for him | 0:26:33 | 0:26:35 | |
to move something that heavy once he's in that position. | 0:26:35 | 0:26:39 | |
Bears have this dexterity and strength | 0:26:40 | 0:26:42 | |
because of the way they feed in the wild. | 0:26:42 | 0:26:46 | |
Perhaps, surprisingly, a lot of their calories come from insects, | 0:26:46 | 0:26:49 | |
which they find by moving boulders and ripping open rotting logs. | 0:26:49 | 0:26:54 | |
All in all, bears are actually well designed for dumpster-driving. | 0:26:57 | 0:27:02 | |
But where was the bear going with its haul? | 0:27:02 | 0:27:05 | |
Well, it turns out it was wheeling it around the corner | 0:27:06 | 0:27:09 | |
into a car park, to feast on the contents. | 0:27:09 | 0:27:13 | |
So, why go for a takeaway each night when it could have dined | 0:27:16 | 0:27:19 | |
al fresco at the back of the restaurant? | 0:27:19 | 0:27:21 | |
Bears, it seems, prefer to dine alone. | 0:27:24 | 0:27:27 | |
They don't like to be caught out unawares either by other | 0:27:27 | 0:27:31 | |
hungry bears or in this case, by humans. | 0:27:31 | 0:27:33 | |
All the activity and the bright lights at the back | 0:27:35 | 0:27:38 | |
of the restaurant are enough to put a bear off his food, | 0:27:38 | 0:27:43 | |
so he takes his meal-on-wheels to a quieter spot in the car park. | 0:27:43 | 0:27:47 | |
But why is there a spike in bear crimes in late summer, | 0:27:50 | 0:27:54 | |
when both of these events occur? | 0:27:54 | 0:27:56 | |
It's all because bears have to hibernate each winter. | 0:27:57 | 0:28:00 | |
Before they can do this, they must pile on the pounds | 0:28:01 | 0:28:05 | |
so they have enough fat to live off during the cold months. | 0:28:05 | 0:28:09 | |
From the end of July onwards, they all start binge eating. | 0:28:09 | 0:28:13 | |
That's what triggers the late summer crime wave. | 0:28:14 | 0:28:17 | |
It certainly provides the motive for that raid on the chocolate shop. | 0:28:19 | 0:28:24 | |
Last week, a young black bear broke into | 0:28:24 | 0:28:26 | |
Rocky Mountain Chocolate Factory in Estes Park and had a heyday stuffing | 0:28:26 | 0:28:30 | |
himself silly full of chocolate treats from the popular candy shop. | 0:28:30 | 0:28:34 | |
Seven times the bear came in, took a pile of chocolate | 0:28:35 | 0:28:39 | |
and went outside to eat it, getting a massive sugar hit. | 0:28:39 | 0:28:43 | |
And there's a theory for why the bears in Colorado, | 0:28:48 | 0:28:50 | |
a landlocked state, | 0:28:50 | 0:28:53 | |
might be more prone to pilfering than bears living near the ocean. | 0:28:53 | 0:28:57 | |
In this sort of an environment in the interior of the continent, | 0:28:57 | 0:29:00 | |
they don't necessarily have access to salmon like they would on the | 0:29:00 | 0:29:03 | |
coast, so their main source of food at this time of year | 0:29:03 | 0:29:06 | |
would be berries. | 0:29:06 | 0:29:08 | |
And berry crops are less nutritious than fish, | 0:29:08 | 0:29:11 | |
and notoriously unpredictable. | 0:29:11 | 0:29:14 | |
One year they might be plentiful, the next, they could be thin | 0:29:15 | 0:29:18 | |
on the ground, driving hungry bears into town to stock up before winter. | 0:29:18 | 0:29:23 | |
And just one last thing that explains why our bear is | 0:29:25 | 0:29:29 | |
so confidently calling for a takeout. | 0:29:29 | 0:29:31 | |
Just look at the way he approaches the dumpster that first night. | 0:29:34 | 0:29:37 | |
He already knows that this is a giant lunchbox to go. | 0:29:39 | 0:29:44 | |
And he's clearly enjoyed the contents of these before | 0:29:44 | 0:29:48 | |
and that's because he probably learned his larceny many years ago. | 0:29:48 | 0:29:54 | |
Bear cubs learn where to find food from their mothers. | 0:29:55 | 0:29:59 | |
Research suggests that the cubs of females that forage in urban | 0:29:59 | 0:30:02 | |
areas grow up to do the same as their mothers. | 0:30:02 | 0:30:05 | |
And there's increasing evidence that these bears will | 0:30:07 | 0:30:10 | |
head into town even when there's plenty of natural food on offer. | 0:30:10 | 0:30:14 | |
The result is a new, streetwise, bear-gang on the block. | 0:30:15 | 0:30:20 | |
Not only do they have a taste for fast food, | 0:30:21 | 0:30:24 | |
they've got the brawn, the brains | 0:30:24 | 0:30:26 | |
and the light-fingeredness to get at it all. | 0:30:26 | 0:30:29 | |
Our bear's midnight feast came to an abrupt end | 0:30:32 | 0:30:35 | |
when the owners of the Edelweiss restaurant bear-proofed their | 0:30:35 | 0:30:38 | |
dumpster and cut off his supply of schnitzel and black forest gateaux. | 0:30:38 | 0:30:43 | |
But then biologists quite often have to relocate thieving bears | 0:30:43 | 0:30:47 | |
into the wilderness. | 0:30:47 | 0:30:48 | |
Trouble is, this doesn't always work. | 0:30:48 | 0:30:51 | |
You see, these animals have extraordinary navigational ability. | 0:30:51 | 0:30:55 | |
Even the cubs can build quite complex mental maps far | 0:30:55 | 0:30:59 | |
better than our own. It's like they've got their own sat-nav. | 0:30:59 | 0:31:02 | |
So if you take them 60, 70 miles away, | 0:31:02 | 0:31:05 | |
within a day or two, they can be right back in town. | 0:31:05 | 0:31:09 | |
But the quest for food takes our next animal | 0:31:10 | 0:31:13 | |
into even more uncharted territory. | 0:31:13 | 0:31:16 | |
It's life and death on the waterways of Southern Europe. | 0:31:17 | 0:31:21 | |
This is Albi, a beautiful town in the South of France. | 0:31:24 | 0:31:28 | |
It's steeped in history, and famous for the picturesque bridges | 0:31:29 | 0:31:34 | |
which cross over the River Tarn. | 0:31:34 | 0:31:36 | |
But these waters hide a shocking secret. | 0:31:37 | 0:31:40 | |
When pigeons come to drink and bathe on the river bank, | 0:31:42 | 0:31:45 | |
they are dicing with death. | 0:31:45 | 0:31:46 | |
A predator is watching their every move. | 0:31:48 | 0:31:51 | |
And French scientists are capturing astonishing events, | 0:31:53 | 0:31:57 | |
never seen before. | 0:31:57 | 0:31:59 | |
Hundreds of miles away in the Spanish city of Zaragoza, | 0:32:12 | 0:32:15 | |
people are stopping in their tracks to watch the same disturbing scenes. | 0:32:15 | 0:32:20 | |
THEY SPEAK SPANISH | 0:32:20 | 0:32:23 | |
One by one, the unsuspecting birds are being plucked from the bank. | 0:32:26 | 0:32:31 | |
So what is preying on these pigeons and dragging them to a watery grave? | 0:32:33 | 0:32:37 | |
Huge fish. | 0:32:39 | 0:32:42 | |
But surely they're risking their own lives to leap onto land? | 0:32:42 | 0:32:46 | |
So what kind of freakish fish would do something so unnatural? | 0:32:46 | 0:32:51 | |
Closer inspection reveals a monstrous head with long | 0:32:51 | 0:32:55 | |
protruding feelers. | 0:32:55 | 0:32:57 | |
It's a colossal kind of catfish and this species | 0:32:57 | 0:33:01 | |
isn't meant to be here at all. | 0:33:01 | 0:33:04 | |
In the 1980s, fishing enthusiasts brought these catfish | 0:33:05 | 0:33:09 | |
from colder climes, introducing them to several parts of Southern Europe. | 0:33:09 | 0:33:14 | |
Since then, their numbers have grown steadily, but this is the first | 0:33:14 | 0:33:19 | |
time they've ever been seen lunging onto land to catch birds. | 0:33:19 | 0:33:23 | |
To explain why they're making such a splash in the south of France, | 0:33:25 | 0:33:28 | |
we need to understand how they normally find food, something | 0:33:28 | 0:33:32 | |
that freshwater fish expert, Jeremy Wade, knows a lot about. | 0:33:32 | 0:33:37 | |
People tend to think of European catfish as being fairly | 0:33:38 | 0:33:42 | |
sluggish creatures, snuffling around on the bottom, | 0:33:42 | 0:33:45 | |
eating things like worms, crustaceans and mussels. | 0:33:45 | 0:33:49 | |
Normally they live in quite muddy water, | 0:33:49 | 0:33:52 | |
that's why they've got those feelers stretched out in front of them. | 0:33:52 | 0:33:56 | |
They have their eyes set towards the top of their head, | 0:33:56 | 0:33:59 | |
they're very aware of what's going on above them. | 0:33:59 | 0:34:02 | |
They're very opportunistic so they'll feed on small fish | 0:34:02 | 0:34:05 | |
but also water birds, things like ducks that are swimming or | 0:34:05 | 0:34:09 | |
floating on the surface, but I've not heard of them taking pigeons before. | 0:34:09 | 0:34:13 | |
Catfish are voracious predators. | 0:34:13 | 0:34:15 | |
To catch their prey, they sneak up behind it. | 0:34:15 | 0:34:19 | |
At the last moment, the catfish opens its mouth creating a huge | 0:34:20 | 0:34:24 | |
inrush of water that literally sucks the creature in alive. | 0:34:24 | 0:34:28 | |
But this won't work with pigeons stood on the bank. | 0:34:30 | 0:34:33 | |
So how have the catfish managed to modify their technique? | 0:34:33 | 0:34:38 | |
The catfish are approaching very stealthily. | 0:34:38 | 0:34:41 | |
They're coming in very close to the water's edge and they appear to | 0:34:41 | 0:34:44 | |
have their feelers stretched out in front of them and what they're doing | 0:34:44 | 0:34:47 | |
with those is feeling vibration. | 0:34:47 | 0:34:50 | |
They seem to be targeting the birds that are moving, | 0:34:50 | 0:34:53 | |
rather than the ones that are standing still. | 0:34:53 | 0:34:56 | |
The prey is not going to come to the fish, | 0:34:56 | 0:34:58 | |
the fish has to go to the prey, which in this case involves | 0:34:58 | 0:35:01 | |
launching itself on land, making a lunge, a grab. | 0:35:01 | 0:35:04 | |
These catfish are doing something rarely seen | 0:35:05 | 0:35:08 | |
anywhere in the world. | 0:35:08 | 0:35:09 | |
They are propelling themselves into elite company, | 0:35:09 | 0:35:13 | |
alongside one of nature's most formidable hunters. | 0:35:13 | 0:35:16 | |
Killer whales off the Patagonian coast also beach themselves | 0:35:21 | 0:35:25 | |
to catch young sea lions. | 0:35:25 | 0:35:27 | |
This spectacular behaviour is a risky tactic, | 0:35:29 | 0:35:34 | |
but at least if they get stuck on the beach, | 0:35:34 | 0:35:36 | |
these mammals are able to breathe air and the reward | 0:35:36 | 0:35:40 | |
of a high protein meal is well worth the risk. | 0:35:40 | 0:35:44 | |
The European catfish are taking the same gamble by hunting pigeons | 0:35:46 | 0:35:50 | |
but the risks are even greater. | 0:35:50 | 0:35:52 | |
They can't breathe air and they could end up beached. | 0:35:52 | 0:35:56 | |
It's the chance of catching the pigeon that makes it all worthwhile. | 0:35:58 | 0:36:02 | |
But French researchers have found that not all | 0:36:04 | 0:36:07 | |
the catfish are making the leap onto land. | 0:36:07 | 0:36:09 | |
It's just a select group of medium sized fish, | 0:36:11 | 0:36:15 | |
ones which are about a metre long. | 0:36:15 | 0:36:17 | |
So why do only these fish hunt the pigeons? | 0:36:19 | 0:36:23 | |
You've got a very gently sloping riverbed there. | 0:36:23 | 0:36:26 | |
The medium sized fish are able to get within range of the pigeons | 0:36:27 | 0:36:31 | |
without the pigeons realising they're there. | 0:36:31 | 0:36:33 | |
A bigger fish tries that, actually half its head is going to be | 0:36:33 | 0:36:36 | |
out of the water. | 0:36:36 | 0:36:38 | |
Before it gets close, that pigeon's going to | 0:36:38 | 0:36:40 | |
realise something's up and get out of the way. | 0:36:40 | 0:36:42 | |
So these medium sized fish are doing it because they can. | 0:36:42 | 0:36:46 | |
But hunger may also be driving them to find food on land. | 0:36:46 | 0:36:50 | |
Maybe the bigger fish are running the river, they are the ones that | 0:36:50 | 0:36:54 | |
have first choice of the normal foodstuffs, but you have this | 0:36:54 | 0:36:57 | |
interesting situation where the medium sized fish have access | 0:36:57 | 0:37:00 | |
to something else that the bigger fish don't seem to be able to reach. | 0:37:00 | 0:37:05 | |
28% of the lunges result in a meal. | 0:37:05 | 0:37:09 | |
That's a good hit rate for any predator. | 0:37:09 | 0:37:13 | |
Fuelled by a high protein diet, these medium sized catfish look set | 0:37:13 | 0:37:18 | |
to join the big boys pretty quickly. | 0:37:18 | 0:37:20 | |
And the irony is, as soon as they grow, pigeon will be off the menu. | 0:37:21 | 0:37:27 | |
But all of this begs the question. | 0:37:28 | 0:37:30 | |
Given the benefits of this feathered food supply, | 0:37:30 | 0:37:33 | |
why haven't catfish done this before, in more locations? | 0:37:33 | 0:37:37 | |
It's to do with where they are. | 0:37:38 | 0:37:40 | |
An introduced species, these cold blooded killers are now | 0:37:40 | 0:37:44 | |
living in warmer climes for the first time. | 0:37:44 | 0:37:47 | |
What I think is probably a factor here is water temperature. | 0:37:47 | 0:37:50 | |
The water in this part of France is significantly warmer | 0:37:50 | 0:37:53 | |
than in their historical home range and what this is going to do | 0:37:53 | 0:37:57 | |
is raise their metabolic rate. | 0:37:57 | 0:37:58 | |
It's going to make them more active, more predatory in their behaviour, | 0:37:58 | 0:38:02 | |
they eat more, they grow bigger, faster. | 0:38:02 | 0:38:04 | |
So, there's a good chance that pigeons will be a la carte | 0:38:07 | 0:38:10 | |
in more places across Southern Europe as catfish numbers increase. | 0:38:10 | 0:38:14 | |
To take advantage of their new home, these fish have shown | 0:38:16 | 0:38:19 | |
remarkable ingenuity, completely changing the way they behave. | 0:38:19 | 0:38:24 | |
They may look primitive, | 0:38:25 | 0:38:26 | |
but catfish are really amazingly adaptable animals. | 0:38:26 | 0:38:31 | |
In the wild, they live for decades and they grow to a colossal size. | 0:38:32 | 0:38:37 | |
The European record was a fish caught in Italy. | 0:38:37 | 0:38:39 | |
It measured nearly 3m in length and weighed in at a staggering 144kg. | 0:38:39 | 0:38:47 | |
It's no wonder that there are stories out there | 0:38:47 | 0:38:49 | |
about these animals feeding on water birds the size of swans | 0:38:49 | 0:38:54 | |
and even swallowing dogs that were swimming in fresh water. | 0:38:54 | 0:38:58 | |
They truly are, if a little unexpectedly, monsters of the deep. | 0:38:58 | 0:39:02 | |
Big fish have crossed a line in Europe to hunt birds, | 0:39:05 | 0:39:09 | |
but in our next story in Israel, the tables are dramatically turned. | 0:39:09 | 0:39:15 | |
Not far from Tel Aviv, in a city park, | 0:39:20 | 0:39:25 | |
the local hooded crows have been doing something unheard of. | 0:39:25 | 0:39:29 | |
Like many urban birds, they'll happily take a hand-out, | 0:39:31 | 0:39:35 | |
but these crows aren't settling for crumbs. | 0:39:35 | 0:39:38 | |
Local resident Oren Hasson | 0:39:38 | 0:39:41 | |
enjoys watching their everyday antics in the park. | 0:39:41 | 0:39:46 | |
Then, one day, Oren filmed a crow taking a piece of bread. | 0:39:46 | 0:39:51 | |
It dropped it into the water and then... | 0:39:51 | 0:39:57 | |
Yes, yes, that crow just caught a live fish. | 0:39:57 | 0:40:03 | |
Crows have never been seen catching fish before and what's really | 0:40:03 | 0:40:07 | |
extraordinary, is that they appear to be using bait to lure them in. | 0:40:07 | 0:40:13 | |
So how have these crows learned to fish for a living? | 0:40:15 | 0:40:20 | |
First, we need to know more about the bird family that they belong to. | 0:40:20 | 0:40:26 | |
The corvids. | 0:40:26 | 0:40:28 | |
Many of the corvids have a reputation for their curiosity | 0:40:30 | 0:40:34 | |
and their problem-solving abilities, and I hope to demonstrate that now | 0:40:34 | 0:40:38 | |
with this raven, because underneath here I've got a test for him. | 0:40:38 | 0:40:42 | |
Now go on, jump down. | 0:40:42 | 0:40:43 | |
Immediately, he's started pulling out the string | 0:40:43 | 0:40:48 | |
because he knows that on the end of that is a ball | 0:40:48 | 0:40:52 | |
with some food in it and now he's got the reward. | 0:40:52 | 0:40:56 | |
Pretty smart, eh? | 0:40:56 | 0:40:58 | |
And remarkably, there are other cases of corvids in the wild using | 0:40:59 | 0:41:04 | |
ingenious techniques to find food. | 0:41:04 | 0:41:07 | |
These rooks have made a pit stop at a motorway service | 0:41:07 | 0:41:10 | |
station in the UK. | 0:41:10 | 0:41:12 | |
They've worked out how to get food from the very | 0:41:12 | 0:41:14 | |
bottom of the litter bins without risking getting inside. | 0:41:14 | 0:41:19 | |
They use the bin liner as a tool, pulling it up to haul the food out. | 0:41:19 | 0:41:24 | |
So, birds like crows can be pretty clever. | 0:41:26 | 0:41:30 | |
But how have the crows in Israel developed their angling abilities? | 0:41:32 | 0:41:36 | |
Many birds dunk dry bread into water to moisten it before they eat it. | 0:41:38 | 0:41:42 | |
Perhaps one accidentally ended up with a fish in its mouth. | 0:41:44 | 0:41:47 | |
That crow might have realised that the fish was a far better meal | 0:41:49 | 0:41:53 | |
and got hooked on fishing. | 0:41:53 | 0:41:55 | |
But then several other crows were deliberately seen using | 0:41:56 | 0:42:00 | |
bread as bait. | 0:42:00 | 0:42:01 | |
Cambridge Crow expert, Nicky Clayton, | 0:42:04 | 0:42:07 | |
thinks there's another intriguing possibility. | 0:42:07 | 0:42:09 | |
Was the bird able to figure out that bread would make a good tool | 0:42:09 | 0:42:13 | |
to use to lure the fish? | 0:42:13 | 0:42:15 | |
Did it have a plan, a series of steps in its head that it's worked out | 0:42:15 | 0:42:20 | |
before it's actually executed any of the behaviours? | 0:42:20 | 0:42:24 | |
So could these crows be thinking through a clear strategy? | 0:42:24 | 0:42:29 | |
What's intriguing about the video is whether the hooded crows | 0:42:31 | 0:42:35 | |
are actually capable of delayed gratification. | 0:42:35 | 0:42:39 | |
That is do they understand that by using a piece of bread | 0:42:39 | 0:42:43 | |
as a tool to lure the fish, they can actually get a much better reward | 0:42:43 | 0:42:47 | |
later, the fish, rather than just eating the piece of bread right now. | 0:42:47 | 0:42:51 | |
Delayed gratification for an animal like this crow is no mean | 0:42:53 | 0:42:57 | |
achievement, particularly as it's not something that comes easily | 0:42:57 | 0:43:01 | |
to humans. | 0:43:01 | 0:43:02 | |
Child psychologists have known for a long time that | 0:43:02 | 0:43:05 | |
if you offer children under five one marshmallow now, | 0:43:05 | 0:43:10 | |
or two marshmallows later and then you leave one on a plate for 15 | 0:43:10 | 0:43:15 | |
minutes, by the end of that period it's almost invariably gone. | 0:43:15 | 0:43:20 | |
And who can blame them! | 0:43:23 | 0:43:25 | |
When crows resist temptation like this, | 0:43:29 | 0:43:32 | |
they also reveal another remarkable ability. | 0:43:32 | 0:43:35 | |
They've got an awareness of time, and that's something, | 0:43:35 | 0:43:40 | |
planning ahead, remembering the past that was thought | 0:43:40 | 0:43:43 | |
until quite recently to be unique to human beings. | 0:43:43 | 0:43:48 | |
So, crows can make and learn to use tools to help them find food. | 0:43:48 | 0:43:54 | |
And they're capable of forward planning and delayed gratification. | 0:43:54 | 0:43:58 | |
So how is all of this possible for a bird brain? | 0:44:00 | 0:44:03 | |
It turns out that crows are a cut above most of their feathered | 0:44:06 | 0:44:10 | |
friends when it comes to the old grey matter. | 0:44:10 | 0:44:12 | |
When we look at the structure of crow brains, | 0:44:14 | 0:44:16 | |
they have areas which are enlarged compared to other birds | 0:44:16 | 0:44:20 | |
and these same areas are the ones that in our brains relate to | 0:44:20 | 0:44:25 | |
memory, planning ahead and delayed gratification. | 0:44:25 | 0:44:29 | |
These amazing new discoveries are leading scientists to make | 0:44:31 | 0:44:34 | |
striking comparisons between crows and great apes. | 0:44:34 | 0:44:38 | |
Relative to their body size, | 0:44:40 | 0:44:42 | |
crows and chimpanzees have equivalent sized brains. | 0:44:42 | 0:44:46 | |
They both use tools and scientific tests are starting to show | 0:44:46 | 0:44:51 | |
that by some criteria, crows are as intelligent as chimpanzees. | 0:44:51 | 0:44:58 | |
Somehow, these very different animals have both developed similar | 0:44:58 | 0:45:02 | |
kinds of brainpower. | 0:45:02 | 0:45:04 | |
It's a mind-blowing thought. | 0:45:09 | 0:45:10 | |
Who'd have guessed that feeding bread to birds in the park | 0:45:12 | 0:45:15 | |
could lead to such startling revelations? | 0:45:15 | 0:45:18 | |
These stories show the extraordinary mental | 0:45:20 | 0:45:23 | |
and physical lengths that animals will go to get a meal. | 0:45:23 | 0:45:27 | |
Whether it's a bear pushing the boundaries, | 0:45:28 | 0:45:33 | |
an astonishing fish out of water, | 0:45:33 | 0:45:36 | |
or a bird brained genius. | 0:45:36 | 0:45:39 | |
These animal innovators have stretched the very | 0:45:42 | 0:45:45 | |
limits of what their brains and bodies are designed to do. | 0:45:45 | 0:45:48 | |
Next, to the design of some of nature's weirdest defences, | 0:45:51 | 0:45:55 | |
improbable strategies for personal protection or | 0:45:55 | 0:45:58 | |
the survival of a whole species. | 0:45:58 | 0:46:01 | |
From birds acting like lame ducks, to shrinking reptiles. | 0:46:03 | 0:46:08 | |
But we start in the USA with a defence strategy that appears | 0:46:10 | 0:46:14 | |
to have backfired terminally. | 0:46:14 | 0:46:16 | |
From time to time, Americans stumble upon apparently dead bodies. | 0:46:19 | 0:46:23 | |
-Uncle, pick it up! -I ain't touching that thing. -Must be poisoned. | 0:46:26 | 0:46:30 | |
-I don't want to touch stuff like that. -Is it going to bite? | 0:46:30 | 0:46:33 | |
Finding what looks like the corpse of a baby rat can be | 0:46:34 | 0:46:37 | |
an unsettling experience. | 0:46:37 | 0:46:39 | |
Watching one lose the will to live in front of your eyes | 0:46:40 | 0:46:43 | |
is even weirder. | 0:46:43 | 0:46:45 | |
Has it died of fright? | 0:46:48 | 0:46:50 | |
Well, no, because these are North American mammals called | 0:46:53 | 0:46:57 | |
Virginia possums, often known as possums for short. | 0:46:57 | 0:47:00 | |
See this thing? It looks dead, doesn't he? | 0:47:02 | 0:47:06 | |
But he isn't. He's just playing possum. | 0:47:06 | 0:47:09 | |
So what is this possum playing at? | 0:47:12 | 0:47:14 | |
The American expression "playing possum" refers to someone | 0:47:16 | 0:47:20 | |
pretending to be dead, and it was inspired by this behaviour. | 0:47:20 | 0:47:25 | |
This bizarre phenomenon is known as thanatosis. | 0:47:25 | 0:47:29 | |
The possum goes stiff, then opens its mouth | 0:47:29 | 0:47:32 | |
and drools with its tongue hanging out. | 0:47:32 | 0:47:34 | |
Its eyes stay open but its breathing becomes slow and shallow. | 0:47:35 | 0:47:41 | |
Despite appearances, its heart continues to beat at a normal rate. | 0:47:41 | 0:47:45 | |
The possum isn't really shutting down, it's just faking it. | 0:47:46 | 0:47:52 | |
At first glance, this is a fatally flawed defence strategy. | 0:47:52 | 0:47:55 | |
-The possum is in mortal danger. -Oh, my gosh! -So why doesn't it run away? | 0:47:57 | 0:48:04 | |
Well, needless to say, possums are not as daft as they look. | 0:48:06 | 0:48:10 | |
Their natural predators include horned owls, coyotes, | 0:48:11 | 0:48:15 | |
bobcats, foxes and racoons. | 0:48:15 | 0:48:17 | |
But with a top speed of about 7mph, running isn't an option, | 0:48:19 | 0:48:24 | |
so the possum doesn't stand a chance against most of these animals. | 0:48:24 | 0:48:29 | |
Yet pretending to be dead when confronted with something | 0:48:29 | 0:48:32 | |
that wants to eat you still seems like a strange response. | 0:48:32 | 0:48:36 | |
But seeing a coyote hunt a possum suggests how faking death | 0:48:38 | 0:48:42 | |
might be the best way of avoiding real death. | 0:48:42 | 0:48:45 | |
The coyote thinks the possum is dead already | 0:48:49 | 0:48:52 | |
so it doesn't bother to kill it. | 0:48:52 | 0:48:54 | |
It switches from full-on attack mode to a more gentle, | 0:48:56 | 0:49:00 | |
meal-handling behaviour. | 0:49:00 | 0:49:02 | |
But the possum is still on the menu so surely real death is inevitable. | 0:49:03 | 0:49:08 | |
Predators don't always get stuck into a meal straight away. | 0:49:10 | 0:49:14 | |
Some species, like owls and foxes, actually hide food for later. | 0:49:16 | 0:49:21 | |
Anyone keeping chickens will know about this scenario. | 0:49:22 | 0:49:25 | |
The fox takes one carcass away from the coup | 0:49:27 | 0:49:30 | |
and hides it in a food cache, returning for another body. | 0:49:30 | 0:49:35 | |
Substitute a death-feigning possum for a chicken | 0:49:36 | 0:49:39 | |
and it would find itself being abducted to the cache site | 0:49:39 | 0:49:43 | |
and then left. | 0:49:43 | 0:49:44 | |
Left for dead, at which point the possum would make its escape. | 0:49:46 | 0:49:52 | |
The case of the coyote reveals the second reason that | 0:49:53 | 0:49:57 | |
playing dead works. | 0:49:57 | 0:49:59 | |
Predators often kill opportunistically | 0:49:59 | 0:50:02 | |
and if they aren't actually ravenous, | 0:50:02 | 0:50:04 | |
they might lose interest in the prey when it appears lifeless. | 0:50:04 | 0:50:09 | |
That means the possum still has a chance. | 0:50:09 | 0:50:12 | |
Whilst the coyote's back is turned, it can slip away, | 0:50:12 | 0:50:16 | |
slightly worse for the experience, but at least it's alive. | 0:50:16 | 0:50:20 | |
So, faking your own death is a risky but effective strategy. | 0:50:25 | 0:50:30 | |
But possums aren't the only animals to evolve what might | 0:50:32 | 0:50:35 | |
seem like counter-intuitive anti-predator strategies. | 0:50:35 | 0:50:39 | |
It happens here in the UK too. These are little ringed plovers. | 0:50:43 | 0:50:49 | |
They lay their eggs on a patch of shingle. | 0:50:49 | 0:50:51 | |
The eggs are speckled for camouflage, but on the ground, | 0:50:52 | 0:50:57 | |
they're still vulnerable to predators like foxes and jackdaws. | 0:50:57 | 0:51:04 | |
This jackdaw knows the eggs are there somewhere, | 0:51:04 | 0:51:07 | |
but the parent birds have left the nest and are flapping | 0:51:07 | 0:51:10 | |
about right under the jackdaw's nose, but this is all a ruse. | 0:51:10 | 0:51:16 | |
Look at the bird in the foreground, | 0:51:16 | 0:51:18 | |
pretending to have a broken wing, trying to look injured | 0:51:18 | 0:51:22 | |
and vulnerable and show that they would be an easy meal. | 0:51:22 | 0:51:26 | |
This is a distraction designed to draw the predator | 0:51:26 | 0:51:29 | |
away from their eggs. | 0:51:29 | 0:51:32 | |
In this case it didn't work. But the strategy is often successful. | 0:51:32 | 0:51:38 | |
As soon as the threat has passed, they drop the act | 0:51:38 | 0:51:41 | |
and return to business as usual. | 0:51:41 | 0:51:43 | |
Birds are likely to perform these distraction displays | 0:51:46 | 0:51:49 | |
at the most critical point in their breeding cycle, | 0:51:49 | 0:51:52 | |
notably when the eggs are just about to hatch. | 0:51:52 | 0:51:55 | |
If they fail there, then they've wasted an entire breeding season. | 0:51:55 | 0:52:00 | |
Oh, and one last note on these bizarre displays. | 0:52:00 | 0:52:03 | |
On land, it's likely that you'll see birds performing them. | 0:52:03 | 0:52:07 | |
Because they're very agile, they can fly away if there's any trouble. | 0:52:07 | 0:52:11 | |
Less mobile animals, well, if they practise this sort of deceit | 0:52:11 | 0:52:15 | |
and get it wrong, it could be fatal. | 0:52:15 | 0:52:17 | |
Some of nature's best defences involve going unnoticed. | 0:52:19 | 0:52:24 | |
We're off to Madagascar to discover the ultimate in downsizing. | 0:52:24 | 0:52:28 | |
A huge island off the coast of Africa, Madagascar is a strange | 0:52:30 | 0:52:35 | |
looking land, and home to an impressive array of animal oddities. | 0:52:35 | 0:52:41 | |
Some of the weirdest are the chameleons. | 0:52:43 | 0:52:46 | |
Here, there are more species of these larger than life | 0:52:46 | 0:52:49 | |
lizards than anywhere else on earth. | 0:52:49 | 0:52:52 | |
These eye-catching characters are shaped in the classic | 0:52:52 | 0:52:55 | |
chameleon design. | 0:52:55 | 0:52:57 | |
They're big, bold and famous for their telescoping tongues. | 0:53:00 | 0:53:05 | |
And, of course, for their legendary ability to change colour. | 0:53:13 | 0:53:17 | |
They use this to communicate with each other | 0:53:17 | 0:53:21 | |
and to blend into the background to avoid predators. | 0:53:21 | 0:53:25 | |
But in 2012, scientists were exploring the furthest | 0:53:29 | 0:53:32 | |
fringes of Madagascar when they discovered this. | 0:53:32 | 0:53:37 | |
A tiny chameleon, and the world's smallest reptile. | 0:53:37 | 0:53:42 | |
It was so astonishing that it made headline news. | 0:53:43 | 0:53:47 | |
It measures just 29mm, and this is a fully grown adult. | 0:53:51 | 0:53:58 | |
This strange new species, named Brookesia micra, | 0:53:58 | 0:54:02 | |
is a fraction of the size of its flashy cousins. | 0:54:02 | 0:54:05 | |
So why would a lizard adopt this extreme miniaturised design? | 0:54:08 | 0:54:13 | |
Frank Glaw, of the Zoological State Collection in Munich, | 0:54:15 | 0:54:18 | |
made this diminutive discovery and he was instantly intrigued. | 0:54:18 | 0:54:23 | |
It's always a great exciting feeling if you discover new species, | 0:54:23 | 0:54:28 | |
especially if you can immediately recognise it as new. | 0:54:28 | 0:54:31 | |
In this case, it was so small, it was immediately clear | 0:54:31 | 0:54:35 | |
that it is different from all the other dwarf Brookesias | 0:54:35 | 0:54:40 | |
and so it was very exciting to have this small animal in our hands. | 0:54:40 | 0:54:45 | |
Brookesia micra belongs to a family of highly specialised | 0:54:46 | 0:54:50 | |
dwarf chameleons. | 0:54:50 | 0:54:52 | |
These little lizards have found a niche down in the leaf | 0:54:54 | 0:54:57 | |
litter of Madagascar's forests. | 0:54:57 | 0:54:59 | |
Scientists think there may be less predators here, and certainly | 0:55:01 | 0:55:05 | |
their dull brown colouring means they're very well camouflaged. | 0:55:05 | 0:55:09 | |
There are more of these dwarf chameleon species in Madagascar | 0:55:11 | 0:55:15 | |
than anywhere else in the world. | 0:55:15 | 0:55:17 | |
But why is Madagascar such a hot spot for these | 0:55:20 | 0:55:24 | |
incredibly tiny chameleons? | 0:55:24 | 0:55:27 | |
What is it about this place that might favour a tiny design? | 0:55:27 | 0:55:32 | |
Well, it's possible that island life tends to promote the little guy, | 0:55:32 | 0:55:37 | |
something known as insular dwarfism. | 0:55:37 | 0:55:39 | |
It suggests that when a normal sized species is | 0:55:41 | 0:55:44 | |
trapped on an island or on an isolated patch of land, | 0:55:44 | 0:55:48 | |
over time, it actually evolves into a smaller creature. | 0:55:48 | 0:55:51 | |
This shrinking process might happen if food was in short supply. | 0:55:53 | 0:55:57 | |
Only the smaller individuals will survive, | 0:55:59 | 0:56:02 | |
because they can get by on less. | 0:56:02 | 0:56:03 | |
When they reproduce, they'll have smaller offspring, | 0:56:05 | 0:56:08 | |
and so on and so on. | 0:56:08 | 0:56:11 | |
Down-sizing, then, can be a long term survival plan. | 0:56:11 | 0:56:14 | |
OK, so that might explain how the other dwarf | 0:56:16 | 0:56:19 | |
chameleons on Madagascar's mainland came about, | 0:56:19 | 0:56:23 | |
but Brookesia micra was found on a little island | 0:56:23 | 0:56:27 | |
off of Madagascar's north coast. | 0:56:27 | 0:56:29 | |
Here, resources are even more limited. | 0:56:30 | 0:56:34 | |
We have the large island of Madagascar that favoured | 0:56:34 | 0:56:36 | |
evolution of tiny Brookesias, and then | 0:56:36 | 0:56:39 | |
we have tiny offshore islands and so we could expect that natural | 0:56:39 | 0:56:44 | |
selection will favour even more tiny chameleons than on the mainland. | 0:56:44 | 0:56:49 | |
So, Brookesia micra's diminutive design may be | 0:56:49 | 0:56:53 | |
the result of a double dose of down-sizing. | 0:56:53 | 0:56:55 | |
This remarkable little animal has yet to reveal all of its secrets, | 0:56:57 | 0:57:01 | |
and, incredibly, scientists think there's a chance that these | 0:57:01 | 0:57:06 | |
chameleons may get even smaller in the future. | 0:57:06 | 0:57:08 | |
Shrinking yourself as a defence strategy is certainly pretty weird, | 0:57:10 | 0:57:13 | |
but then again it's working for the miniature chameleons of Madagascar. | 0:57:13 | 0:57:18 | |
The question is though, just how much smaller could they get? | 0:57:18 | 0:57:21 | |
Well, it appears that there might be a limit to miniaturisation | 0:57:21 | 0:57:26 | |
governed by the complexity of one organ. | 0:57:26 | 0:57:28 | |
It's not the heart, it's not the lungs, it's not the liver, | 0:57:28 | 0:57:32 | |
it is in fact, the eye. | 0:57:32 | 0:57:34 | |
As these stories show us, many of nature's defence plans | 0:57:36 | 0:57:39 | |
are stranger than we could possibly imagine. | 0:57:39 | 0:57:42 | |
Whether it's a possum faking its own death, | 0:57:44 | 0:57:47 | |
birds deploying distraction techniques | 0:57:47 | 0:57:53 | |
or lizards shrinking into the background, | 0:57:53 | 0:57:58 | |
they're all strange and curious designs | 0:57:58 | 0:58:00 | |
showing nature at its weirdest. | 0:58:00 | 0:58:03 | |
So there you have it. | 0:58:05 | 0:58:06 | |
We have witnessed some shocking events, | 0:58:06 | 0:58:09 | |
uncovered some fascinating new science | 0:58:09 | 0:58:12 | |
and also seen the weird and the wonderful. | 0:58:12 | 0:58:15 | |
And with its bizarre animal behaviour | 0:58:15 | 0:58:18 | |
and strange natural phenomena, | 0:58:18 | 0:58:20 | |
there's no doubt at all that the natural world still has | 0:58:20 | 0:58:23 | |
the power to leave us both baffled and amazed. | 0:58:23 | 0:58:28 | |
And with so much more to discover, | 0:58:28 | 0:58:30 | |
it also leaves us with one last question. | 0:58:30 | 0:58:34 | |
What on earth next? | 0:58:34 | 0:58:35 |