Browse content similar to Using Sound. Check below for episodes and series from the same categories and more!
Line | From | To | |
---|---|---|---|
TWEETING BIRDS | 0:00:02 | 0:00:04 | |
BUZZING BEES | 0:00:05 | 0:00:07 | |
This is a familiar scene. | 0:00:19 | 0:00:21 | |
It's the Somerset countryside on a calm day. | 0:00:21 | 0:00:24 | |
And it sounds familiar. | 0:00:24 | 0:00:26 | |
I can hear the birds singing, | 0:00:26 | 0:00:27 | |
I can hear the wind rustling through the trees | 0:00:27 | 0:00:30 | |
and I can hear the insects around me. | 0:00:30 | 0:00:32 | |
This isn't just a landscape, it's a soundscape. | 0:00:32 | 0:00:35 | |
A constant flood of sound waves washing over me from all directions. | 0:00:35 | 0:00:40 | |
'No matter where we are or where we go, sound is always present. | 0:00:47 | 0:00:53 | |
'And each individual noise offers us information about our world | 0:00:55 | 0:01:01 | |
'from a moment in time and space. | 0:01:01 | 0:01:04 | |
'Every sound wave carries a story about where it's come from | 0:01:04 | 0:01:08 | |
'and the journey it's been on.' | 0:01:08 | 0:01:10 | |
And our evolutionary history has given us these two detectors | 0:01:10 | 0:01:13 | |
for tapping into those stories. | 0:01:13 | 0:01:16 | |
What we hear shapes our understanding of our world. | 0:01:16 | 0:01:20 | |
'In this programme, I'm going to explore how we exploit, | 0:01:24 | 0:01:27 | |
'manipulate and control sound.' | 0:01:27 | 0:01:30 | |
Just the quality of the sound says something's not right in here. | 0:01:32 | 0:01:36 | |
'I'll delve into the complex ways in which our own bodies | 0:01:36 | 0:01:39 | |
'precisely decode the information carried in sound waves.' | 0:01:39 | 0:01:44 | |
That's amazing. | 0:01:48 | 0:01:49 | |
When you take it off I can hear nothing. | 0:01:49 | 0:01:52 | |
It's incredible! | 0:01:52 | 0:01:54 | |
'And how the more we've come to understand sound, | 0:01:57 | 0:02:00 | |
'the more we've been able to use it | 0:02:00 | 0:02:03 | |
'to make remarkable discoveries about life... | 0:02:03 | 0:02:06 | |
'..our planet... | 0:02:07 | 0:02:09 | |
'..and even the solar system.' | 0:02:11 | 0:02:13 | |
'In our normal everyday lives, it's hard to really appreciate | 0:02:29 | 0:02:34 | |
'how much information sound carries.' | 0:02:34 | 0:02:37 | |
-Want to put the helmet on? -OK. -You need those, as well. | 0:02:39 | 0:02:42 | |
'Which is why acoustic engineer Professor Trevor Cox is taking me | 0:02:42 | 0:02:46 | |
'to a hidden location deep inside the hills of Scotland. | 0:02:46 | 0:02:51 | |
'Where, in the absence of light, hearing becomes my primary sense.' | 0:02:57 | 0:03:02 | |
I'm going to go in first, so I shall demonstrate. | 0:03:07 | 0:03:10 | |
It's ever so slightly sinister, this, isn't it? | 0:03:16 | 0:03:18 | |
-There's your helmet. -OK. -You want to put your gloves on. | 0:03:18 | 0:03:21 | |
I've probably have nightmares about doing something like this. | 0:03:21 | 0:03:24 | |
Slide yourself in. | 0:03:24 | 0:03:25 | |
Now, just be really careful as you get up. A bit further. | 0:03:31 | 0:03:35 | |
-OK? -What have I arrived into? | 0:03:36 | 0:03:39 | |
I'm going to be slightly cruel and turn my head torch off | 0:03:39 | 0:03:41 | |
so we can't really see. | 0:03:41 | 0:03:43 | |
We're just really working with the acoustic here. | 0:03:43 | 0:03:46 | |
You have one of those. Shall we wander in just a bit further? | 0:03:46 | 0:03:48 | |
Very, very dark, isn't it? | 0:03:48 | 0:03:50 | |
-Watch where you walk. -Urgh, that's horrid. | 0:03:50 | 0:03:52 | |
This is where the baddie turns up, right? | 0:03:52 | 0:03:55 | |
Two people walk into a dark space | 0:03:55 | 0:03:57 | |
and just the quality of the sound says something's not right here. | 0:03:57 | 0:04:01 | |
'Just from the way that sounds behave in this place, | 0:04:01 | 0:04:04 | |
'I'm beginning to piece together a picture of what it might be like.' | 0:04:04 | 0:04:08 | |
What do you think this space is? | 0:04:08 | 0:04:10 | |
So, it feels like it's gigantic. | 0:04:10 | 0:04:13 | |
I can't tell because I can't see anything but it feels as though | 0:04:13 | 0:04:16 | |
it could be enormous - the size of a cathedral or bigger. | 0:04:16 | 0:04:19 | |
Just because that's the only place | 0:04:19 | 0:04:22 | |
I've heard this sort of thing happen to my voice before. | 0:04:22 | 0:04:26 | |
I'm finding it hard to finish a sentence because I keep saying | 0:04:27 | 0:04:30 | |
a word and then stopping to listen to what it sounds like. | 0:04:30 | 0:04:34 | |
When you listen to a sound in a room you can get a lot of information. | 0:04:34 | 0:04:37 | |
You'll get the sound straight from me to you | 0:04:37 | 0:04:40 | |
and then all the walls are contributing reflections - | 0:04:40 | 0:04:43 | |
the sound's bouncing around the room. | 0:04:43 | 0:04:45 | |
All the time in a space we're listening for these sort of clues. | 0:04:45 | 0:04:48 | |
But we're not usually that, you know, conscious we're doing it. | 0:04:48 | 0:04:51 | |
'The ability of sound to reflect is one of the most critical ways | 0:04:51 | 0:04:55 | |
'it can carry information. | 0:04:55 | 0:04:58 | |
'But sound reflections can tell me more than the size of a place. | 0:04:58 | 0:05:02 | |
'I just need a different type of sound.' | 0:05:02 | 0:05:05 | |
-I've got a stopwatch for you there. -OK. | 0:05:05 | 0:05:07 | |
-So, if you could wait for... hear the bang. -Yeah. | 0:05:07 | 0:05:10 | |
And then just measure how long it takes the sound to decay to nothing, | 0:05:10 | 0:05:14 | |
which is actually how they first measured reverberation. | 0:05:14 | 0:05:17 | |
-I shall retreat to a safe distance. -Yeah! | 0:05:17 | 0:05:19 | |
I just dropped it. | 0:05:36 | 0:05:38 | |
I can't see... | 0:05:39 | 0:05:41 | |
LOUD BANG | 0:05:44 | 0:05:46 | |
SOUND SLOWLY DIMINISHES | 0:05:47 | 0:05:50 | |
57 seconds. | 0:06:06 | 0:06:08 | |
Wow. | 0:06:09 | 0:06:11 | |
This place actually holds the world record | 0:06:11 | 0:06:13 | |
for the longest reverberation time, | 0:06:13 | 0:06:15 | |
which is what you kind of measured there. | 0:06:15 | 0:06:18 | |
What's going on to make that happen? | 0:06:18 | 0:06:20 | |
First of all, it's a very big place. | 0:06:20 | 0:06:22 | |
But there must be something more than that | 0:06:22 | 0:06:24 | |
because if you go into St Paul's Cathedral in London, | 0:06:24 | 0:06:26 | |
the sound would only last about ten seconds before dying away. | 0:06:26 | 0:06:29 | |
The sound is being contained and held in this giant space. | 0:06:29 | 0:06:33 | |
And that's because the walls here are incredibly massive. | 0:06:33 | 0:06:36 | |
You can tell that this must have hard, heavy walls, | 0:06:36 | 0:06:39 | |
whereas if you brought a lot of soft furnishings in, | 0:06:39 | 0:06:42 | |
which absorb sound, this place would go dead. | 0:06:42 | 0:06:44 | |
So, we're getting extra information | 0:06:44 | 0:06:46 | |
because sound reflects differently off different materials. | 0:06:46 | 0:06:49 | |
What is this place? After all that, where are we? | 0:06:49 | 0:06:51 | |
Well, let's put the lights on. | 0:06:51 | 0:06:53 | |
So, this is a massive space. | 0:07:06 | 0:07:07 | |
It's about a quarter of a kilometre long | 0:07:07 | 0:07:09 | |
so that's where a lot of the reverberations come. | 0:07:09 | 0:07:12 | |
-What's it doing here? -Well, it's actually an oil storage depot | 0:07:12 | 0:07:15 | |
which was built in the run-up to World War II | 0:07:15 | 0:07:17 | |
to protect the Royal Navy shipping oil from bombing. | 0:07:17 | 0:07:20 | |
So, it's been made bombproof | 0:07:20 | 0:07:22 | |
and that's the reason it's got this huge reverberance. | 0:07:22 | 0:07:25 | |
They've made it out of half-metre-thick concrete | 0:07:25 | 0:07:27 | |
and behind it is the bedrock of Scotland. | 0:07:27 | 0:07:30 | |
So, this is really massive walls. | 0:07:30 | 0:07:32 | |
And the walls are covered in oil, as well. It's horribly sticky. | 0:07:32 | 0:07:34 | |
Sticky on your feet, everywhere. That's really useful acoustically. | 0:07:34 | 0:07:37 | |
Concrete's a bit porous so normally you get a little bit of absorption | 0:07:37 | 0:07:40 | |
but its pores have been gunked up with oil. | 0:07:40 | 0:07:43 | |
So, what's happening is that the sound is reflecting off the walls | 0:07:43 | 0:07:46 | |
really efficiently, it's not getting absorbed. | 0:07:46 | 0:07:48 | |
You can get a tremendous lot of information by looking at | 0:07:48 | 0:07:51 | |
the pattern of reflections, and, as an acoustic engineer, | 0:07:51 | 0:07:54 | |
that's what you do when you design a grand concert hall. | 0:07:54 | 0:07:57 | |
SAXOPHONE REVERBERATES | 0:07:59 | 0:08:02 | |
You try and design the pattern of reflections | 0:08:08 | 0:08:10 | |
to be just right to enhance the music. | 0:08:10 | 0:08:13 | |
SAXOPHONE REVERBERATES | 0:08:14 | 0:08:16 | |
'The reason that sound can carry so much information | 0:08:34 | 0:08:37 | |
'is because of its fundamental nature. | 0:08:37 | 0:08:41 | |
'It travels as a wave. | 0:08:41 | 0:08:43 | |
'And every time a sound wave reflects off a surface | 0:08:44 | 0:08:47 | |
'it's changed in subtle ways.' | 0:08:47 | 0:08:50 | |
Reflection is a way of redirecting sound | 0:08:52 | 0:08:54 | |
and that redirected sound carries information | 0:08:54 | 0:08:57 | |
about the obstacle it bounced off. | 0:08:57 | 0:09:00 | |
We use that acoustic signature to learn about our environment | 0:09:00 | 0:09:04 | |
in a general way, but there are animals that absolutely rely on it, | 0:09:04 | 0:09:07 | |
and they are the true masters of sound. | 0:09:07 | 0:09:10 | |
'For most bats, hearing is their primary sense. | 0:09:31 | 0:09:35 | |
'Listening to sound reflections is key to their survival. | 0:09:35 | 0:09:39 | |
'And their success has driven complex relationships | 0:09:42 | 0:09:45 | |
'with other creatures that live in and exploit this auditory world. | 0:09:45 | 0:09:50 | |
'Bats are one of the loudest creatures in the animal kingdom. | 0:09:54 | 0:09:58 | |
'We can't hear them because they mostly use frequencies | 0:09:58 | 0:10:01 | |
'our ears can't detect, making it quite difficult for bat experts | 0:10:01 | 0:10:05 | |
'like Dr Marc Holderied to study them.' | 0:10:05 | 0:10:08 | |
We have an acoustic camera that can pick up ultrasound | 0:10:08 | 0:10:11 | |
and we've just put it in one of my favourite research spots. | 0:10:11 | 0:10:14 | |
So, this is a commuting corridor | 0:10:14 | 0:10:15 | |
with loads of bats using it every night. | 0:10:15 | 0:10:17 | |
And this acoustic camera now shows me what is going on | 0:10:17 | 0:10:21 | |
as we look at this screen. | 0:10:21 | 0:10:23 | |
We've just seen two bats flying and there's a third one. | 0:10:23 | 0:10:26 | |
So, there's a whole group flying past. | 0:10:26 | 0:10:28 | |
You can see all these whitish yellowish blobs there. | 0:10:28 | 0:10:31 | |
As the bat was flying past | 0:10:31 | 0:10:33 | |
it was emitting these ultrasonic frequencies. | 0:10:33 | 0:10:35 | |
So, you're looking for patterns? | 0:10:35 | 0:10:37 | |
We can look at this spectrogram display down here | 0:10:37 | 0:10:40 | |
and try and find out which species we were looking at. | 0:10:40 | 0:10:43 | |
There's another one coming right now. | 0:10:43 | 0:10:45 | |
Now, if you look at that, they all ended about the same frequency. | 0:10:45 | 0:10:49 | |
They're around 45 kilohertz, | 0:10:49 | 0:10:50 | |
which tells us that this is a common pipistrelle. | 0:10:50 | 0:10:53 | |
And just now is a very different call. | 0:10:53 | 0:10:56 | |
And I can tell you that this is a Daubenton's bat. | 0:10:56 | 0:10:58 | |
So, you're painting this picture of all these bats whooshing past us, | 0:10:58 | 0:11:01 | |
making sounds that we can't hear. | 0:11:01 | 0:11:03 | |
If we could hear them, what would we hear? | 0:11:03 | 0:11:06 | |
What I've brought along here is a tiny bat detector. | 0:11:06 | 0:11:08 | |
It turns the ultrasonic frequencies into audible frequencies. | 0:11:08 | 0:11:12 | |
-That was a bat! -There's one flying over right now. | 0:11:12 | 0:11:15 | |
We heard this very quick succession of calls there. | 0:11:15 | 0:11:18 | |
There it is again. Very good. It just whizzed over there. | 0:11:18 | 0:11:21 | |
So, they're very short and sharp and even though that sounds very quick | 0:11:21 | 0:11:24 | |
-to us, there's a lot going on between one pulse and the next. -Yes. | 0:11:24 | 0:11:27 | |
They send out the high-intensity sound... | 0:11:28 | 0:11:31 | |
..and then they hit all the obstacles that are in the area. | 0:11:32 | 0:11:35 | |
These obstacles produce echoes | 0:11:35 | 0:11:37 | |
and the bat then waits for these to come back. | 0:11:37 | 0:11:40 | |
The further away an object is, the longer the echo takes to return | 0:11:42 | 0:11:45 | |
to the bat and this is how bats measure distance. | 0:11:45 | 0:11:48 | |
And that is an incredibly complex achievement. | 0:11:49 | 0:11:53 | |
There is so many different reflectors, like all the leaves, | 0:11:54 | 0:11:56 | |
you have the ground, you have all the branches, | 0:11:56 | 0:11:59 | |
and all of them produce echoes. | 0:11:59 | 0:12:01 | |
'Bats evolved the ability to use sound to see | 0:12:10 | 0:12:14 | |
'at least 53 million years ago... | 0:12:14 | 0:12:17 | |
'..giving them an enormous advantage when hunting for prey | 0:12:18 | 0:12:21 | |
'under the cover of darkness.' | 0:12:21 | 0:12:23 | |
So, we've got a moth here. What species is it? | 0:12:25 | 0:12:28 | |
It's a heart and dart. | 0:12:28 | 0:12:29 | |
It's got this beautiful gold sheen. | 0:12:29 | 0:12:31 | |
Yeah, yeah, yeah, they are quite beautiful. | 0:12:31 | 0:12:34 | |
And how's a bat going to find this moth? | 0:12:34 | 0:12:36 | |
So, a bat uses biosonar not only for navigation but also to capture prey. | 0:12:36 | 0:12:41 | |
So, when they are searching for insects, they want to look very far. | 0:12:41 | 0:12:45 | |
So, what they use is their lowest frequency calls that carry very far. | 0:12:45 | 0:12:49 | |
But as soon as they've detected the moth, | 0:12:49 | 0:12:51 | |
they add in higher frequencies to their calls. | 0:12:51 | 0:12:54 | |
BAT CALLS | 0:12:54 | 0:12:56 | |
Higher frequencies have shorter wavelength | 0:12:56 | 0:12:59 | |
and give them better resolution. | 0:12:59 | 0:13:01 | |
And better resolution means they can localise the moth very well. | 0:13:01 | 0:13:05 | |
BAT CALLS | 0:13:05 | 0:13:07 | |
And the bat sonar is giving it a brilliant tool | 0:13:09 | 0:13:11 | |
for finding these very fast-moving moths. | 0:13:11 | 0:13:14 | |
Do they have it all their own way? | 0:13:14 | 0:13:16 | |
Moths, of course, are fighting back. | 0:13:16 | 0:13:18 | |
All these moths had to do is evolve an ultrasound sensitive ear | 0:13:18 | 0:13:22 | |
that picks up the frequencies the bats emit, and they did. | 0:13:22 | 0:13:25 | |
-So, can this moth hear? -This moth has ears, yes. | 0:13:25 | 0:13:28 | |
When they hear a bat that's far away, | 0:13:28 | 0:13:30 | |
they just steer out of harm's way. | 0:13:30 | 0:13:32 | |
And so, there's, sort of, one of these arms races going on | 0:13:32 | 0:13:35 | |
where one species makes a change that makes them more successful | 0:13:35 | 0:13:39 | |
and then their prey species also has to adapt. | 0:13:39 | 0:13:41 | |
So... Oh, it's going for a walk again. | 0:13:41 | 0:13:44 | |
And are there any other strategies that a moth could take | 0:13:44 | 0:13:46 | |
-to avoid this bat that's coming to get it for dinner? -Yes. | 0:13:46 | 0:13:49 | |
Moths have taken the next step. | 0:13:49 | 0:13:51 | |
Moths have evolved a jamming mechanism | 0:13:51 | 0:13:55 | |
that helps them throw the biosonar off target. | 0:13:55 | 0:13:59 | |
You have a moth that knows it's under attack, | 0:14:00 | 0:14:02 | |
it produces ultrasonic clicks. | 0:14:02 | 0:14:05 | |
And these ultrasonic clicks are in the similar frequency range | 0:14:05 | 0:14:08 | |
as the echoes a bat is expecting. | 0:14:08 | 0:14:11 | |
But if it hears these clicks rather than the echoes | 0:14:11 | 0:14:14 | |
it can't really make out a full echolocation picture any more. | 0:14:14 | 0:14:18 | |
And that gives the moth the time to just whizz out of the way. | 0:14:19 | 0:14:23 | |
MOTH CLICKS | 0:14:23 | 0:14:25 | |
'This sophisticated interplay between bats and moths | 0:14:31 | 0:14:35 | |
'shows just how rich in information | 0:14:35 | 0:14:38 | |
'and how valuable reflecting sound waves can be. | 0:14:38 | 0:14:42 | |
'But reflections are not the only way sound waves help us | 0:14:42 | 0:14:46 | |
'understand our surroundings. | 0:14:46 | 0:14:48 | |
'There's another feature of sound that can provide us with | 0:14:48 | 0:14:51 | |
'even more information about the world. | 0:14:51 | 0:14:54 | |
'And it's particularly useful in warning us of approaching danger. | 0:14:54 | 0:14:58 | |
CAR HORN | 0:15:00 | 0:15:02 | |
AMBULANCE SIREN | 0:15:05 | 0:15:07 | |
LOUD TRAFFIC NOISES | 0:15:10 | 0:15:12 | |
I live in London and I cycle all the time | 0:15:16 | 0:15:19 | |
and it's easily the most dangerous thing I do on a daily basis. | 0:15:19 | 0:15:23 | |
There's so much traffic here. | 0:15:23 | 0:15:25 | |
Vans like that that overtake you when you're not expecting them. | 0:15:26 | 0:15:30 | |
What I'm conscious of is paying attention to light. | 0:15:32 | 0:15:35 | |
I can see what's in front of me, I look behind me, | 0:15:35 | 0:15:38 | |
that makes me feel secure. | 0:15:38 | 0:15:40 | |
But I'm getting a lot of extra information from sound. | 0:15:40 | 0:15:43 | |
Two things that really worry me when I'm cycling, | 0:15:48 | 0:15:50 | |
and they are big trucks and motorcycles. | 0:15:50 | 0:15:52 | |
Fortunately, both of them make a huge amount of noise. | 0:15:52 | 0:15:55 | |
That was a motorbike. | 0:15:55 | 0:15:57 | |
And you can hear them coming, even from around the corner. | 0:15:57 | 0:16:00 | |
LOUD MOTORCYCLE ENGINE | 0:16:00 | 0:16:02 | |
I certainly heard him. | 0:16:02 | 0:16:04 | |
'We can often hear things we can't see | 0:16:06 | 0:16:09 | |
'because, unlike light, sound can travel around corners. | 0:16:09 | 0:16:13 | |
'It's something made possible when a fundamental feature | 0:16:18 | 0:16:21 | |
'of the sound wave is just right - its size.' | 0:16:21 | 0:16:25 | |
It works a bit like this. | 0:16:25 | 0:16:27 | |
If we imagine we've got an obstacle in the way | 0:16:27 | 0:16:29 | |
and in this case that could be the corner of a building. | 0:16:29 | 0:16:32 | |
I'm going to draw a sound source over here. | 0:16:32 | 0:16:35 | |
Sound is spreading out in ripples, like the ripples on a pond. | 0:16:35 | 0:16:39 | |
So, as the sound travels away, those ripples spread out. | 0:16:39 | 0:16:42 | |
They can spread around the corner. | 0:16:42 | 0:16:44 | |
So, if I was standing here, I might not be able to see the sound source | 0:16:44 | 0:16:48 | |
but I would be able to hear the sound. | 0:16:48 | 0:16:50 | |
And this is called diffraction. | 0:16:50 | 0:16:52 | |
It doesn't work in the same way for all wavelengths | 0:16:52 | 0:16:55 | |
because diffraction depends on how the wavelength | 0:16:55 | 0:16:58 | |
is related to the size of the obstacle. | 0:16:58 | 0:17:00 | |
And a corner of a building is quite big. | 0:17:00 | 0:17:02 | |
So, this time I'm going to draw a higher frequency sound | 0:17:02 | 0:17:06 | |
which means the wavelengths are much shorter. | 0:17:06 | 0:17:09 | |
So, they'll spread out like ripples and they will diffract a little bit | 0:17:09 | 0:17:13 | |
as they go around the corner but not nearly as much. | 0:17:13 | 0:17:17 | |
So, sound that might be a wavelength of a few centimetres | 0:17:17 | 0:17:20 | |
are much smaller than the corner of the building | 0:17:20 | 0:17:22 | |
so I can't hear the high frequencies here | 0:17:22 | 0:17:25 | |
but I can hear the low frequencies. | 0:17:25 | 0:17:27 | |
'Most sounds can travel around objects | 0:17:31 | 0:17:33 | |
'because their wavelength is relatively big. | 0:17:33 | 0:17:36 | |
'Light, on the other hand, has a very short wavelength. | 0:17:37 | 0:17:40 | |
'Which means there are very few things in our world | 0:17:43 | 0:17:45 | |
'that it can bend around. | 0:17:45 | 0:17:47 | |
'Instead, light stops and casts a shadow. | 0:17:47 | 0:17:51 | |
'The ease with which sound can travel around the environment | 0:17:59 | 0:18:03 | |
'has played an important role in the story of our survival. | 0:18:03 | 0:18:07 | |
'Because it means we can hear the roar of a hungry lion | 0:18:08 | 0:18:11 | |
'or the rumble of a truck - even if we can't see them.' | 0:18:11 | 0:18:15 | |
The diffraction of sound does more than just let me know | 0:18:15 | 0:18:18 | |
that there's a sound source somewhere near me. | 0:18:18 | 0:18:21 | |
It helps me pinpoint exactly where that sound source is. | 0:18:21 | 0:18:25 | |
'This ability is called localisation. | 0:18:33 | 0:18:36 | |
'Every animal needs to know which direction danger is coming from. | 0:18:38 | 0:18:42 | |
'It works because sound doesn't just diffract around our environment, | 0:18:43 | 0:18:47 | |
'but also around the listener. | 0:18:47 | 0:18:50 | |
'Dr Jenny Bizley is here to show me the complex mechanisms | 0:18:55 | 0:18:59 | |
'we use to localise sound.' | 0:18:59 | 0:19:01 | |
So, I don't know where the sound's going to come from? | 0:19:01 | 0:19:04 | |
No, so if you face the front, I'll play a sound | 0:19:04 | 0:19:07 | |
and then you can maybe point to where you think it comes from. | 0:19:07 | 0:19:11 | |
No pressure! | 0:19:11 | 0:19:12 | |
LOUD MONKEY CHATTER Oh, it's loud, isn't it? | 0:19:13 | 0:19:16 | |
-Somewhere over there. -Yeah, that's right. | 0:19:16 | 0:19:18 | |
We'll try another one. | 0:19:18 | 0:19:20 | |
GRUNTING Up there! | 0:19:20 | 0:19:23 | |
Yeah. And how about this one? | 0:19:24 | 0:19:26 | |
LOUD WHOOSHING Somewhere up there. | 0:19:26 | 0:19:29 | |
CRASHING Something broke over there. | 0:19:29 | 0:19:32 | |
'Although I'm not conscious of it, | 0:19:32 | 0:19:34 | |
'my brain is precisely locating each sound I'm hearing.' | 0:19:34 | 0:19:38 | |
So, the biggest bee in the world is over there! | 0:19:38 | 0:19:40 | |
Oh, it's moving. | 0:19:40 | 0:19:42 | |
'And it's not limited to fixed sounds. | 0:19:42 | 0:19:45 | |
'To understand how we localise sound, | 0:19:48 | 0:19:51 | |
'we need to look at the way it moves around our bodies | 0:19:51 | 0:19:55 | |
'and interacts with the two ears on opposite sides of our head.' | 0:19:55 | 0:19:59 | |
So, we're going to play the sound of the twig snapping | 0:20:00 | 0:20:02 | |
that you heard previously from one of the speakers over there, | 0:20:02 | 0:20:05 | |
and it was coming from the left of the head. | 0:20:05 | 0:20:07 | |
And we'll look at the input from the microphones on here. | 0:20:07 | 0:20:09 | |
So, we should see the sound waves coming in here. | 0:20:09 | 0:20:12 | |
TWIG SNAPPING | 0:20:12 | 0:20:13 | |
So, this is the signal from the first microphone, | 0:20:13 | 0:20:16 | |
which is on the left, | 0:20:16 | 0:20:17 | |
and this is the signal from the right microphone. | 0:20:17 | 0:20:19 | |
-And they look very different. -Yes. | 0:20:20 | 0:20:22 | |
You can see that the left-hand microphone is picking up a signal | 0:20:22 | 0:20:25 | |
that's much louder than the signal on the right. | 0:20:25 | 0:20:28 | |
And it's also arriving sooner. | 0:20:28 | 0:20:29 | |
The timing difference, how long is that from there to there? | 0:20:29 | 0:20:32 | |
From there to there is about 500 microseconds. | 0:20:32 | 0:20:36 | |
So, just about half of a millisecond. | 0:20:36 | 0:20:38 | |
So, the sound reached my left ear | 0:20:38 | 0:20:39 | |
-half a millisecond before it reached my right ear? -Yeah. | 0:20:39 | 0:20:42 | |
We can measure that difference | 0:20:42 | 0:20:43 | |
because sound moves relatively slowly, at least compared to light. | 0:20:43 | 0:20:46 | |
The difference in timing is useful for low-frequency sounds. | 0:20:46 | 0:20:50 | |
Because the low-frequency sound has quite a long wavelength, | 0:20:50 | 0:20:53 | |
longer than the width of the head, | 0:20:53 | 0:20:55 | |
the sound can diffract around the head to the far ear, | 0:20:55 | 0:20:58 | |
but it does so with a delay. | 0:20:58 | 0:20:59 | |
The other big difference here is the amplitude - the level of the sound. | 0:20:59 | 0:21:03 | |
What's the level difference between one ear and the other? | 0:21:03 | 0:21:06 | |
For this sound, we have a difference of the order of a few decibels, | 0:21:06 | 0:21:09 | |
5-10, depending on the frequency of the sound. | 0:21:09 | 0:21:12 | |
-So, that's quite a lot, is it? -That's quite a large difference. | 0:21:12 | 0:21:15 | |
The amplitude difference is important really | 0:21:15 | 0:21:17 | |
for high-frequency sounds which have shorter wavelengths. | 0:21:17 | 0:21:20 | |
They are not able to diffract around the head | 0:21:20 | 0:21:23 | |
and they are shadowed by the head. | 0:21:23 | 0:21:25 | |
So, the signal will be louder in the near ear and quieter in the far ear. | 0:21:25 | 0:21:28 | |
These signals are kept within the brain and they're kept separately | 0:21:28 | 0:21:31 | |
until higher up, sort of, in the processing hierarchy, | 0:21:31 | 0:21:34 | |
when they're put together to give you a perception of space. | 0:21:34 | 0:21:37 | |
And that means that, within seconds, you can tell where a sound | 0:21:37 | 0:21:40 | |
comes from so that you can avoid it if it's going to eat you, or... | 0:21:40 | 0:21:43 | |
I definitely avoid sounds that are going to eat me! | 0:21:43 | 0:21:45 | |
'However, this system only works for localising sound | 0:21:49 | 0:21:53 | |
'in the horizontal plane. | 0:21:53 | 0:21:55 | |
'To know whether the sound is coming from above or below, | 0:21:58 | 0:22:02 | |
'we use a trick that depends on the shape of each individual ear. | 0:22:02 | 0:22:06 | |
'To show me, Jenny has kindly brought with her what looks like | 0:22:13 | 0:22:17 | |
'an awful lot of Blu-Tack.' | 0:22:17 | 0:22:19 | |
You know at school, | 0:22:19 | 0:22:21 | |
teachers were always taking Blu-Tack out of people's ears. | 0:22:21 | 0:22:24 | |
Somehow, you get older, and you become a scientist | 0:22:24 | 0:22:26 | |
and it works the other way around. | 0:22:26 | 0:22:28 | |
'The aim is to smooth out the folds of my outer ear.' | 0:22:28 | 0:22:32 | |
There you are. Take your finger out. | 0:22:32 | 0:22:34 | |
OK, now I've got ears full of Plasticine. Brilliant! | 0:22:34 | 0:22:37 | |
Sound can still go down there but it can't bounce off all of this. | 0:22:37 | 0:22:41 | |
I'm going to clap somewhere in front of you | 0:22:41 | 0:22:43 | |
and you should just close your eyes and then point at it. | 0:22:43 | 0:22:46 | |
OK, all right. | 0:22:46 | 0:22:47 | |
Erm, there? | 0:22:48 | 0:22:50 | |
No?! | 0:22:51 | 0:22:52 | |
OK, give me another try, give me another try. Go on. | 0:22:52 | 0:22:55 | |
-Down there? -No. | 0:22:56 | 0:22:58 | |
There? | 0:22:59 | 0:23:01 | |
So, I'm rubbish at this with these in my ears. | 0:23:01 | 0:23:03 | |
I'm going to take these out because they're doing... | 0:23:03 | 0:23:06 | |
It does make the world sound very weird, actually. | 0:23:06 | 0:23:08 | |
When I've got them in, it's like there's less going on | 0:23:08 | 0:23:12 | |
and I take them out and suddenly the world opens out. | 0:23:12 | 0:23:14 | |
You're just missing that information that you're used to having. | 0:23:14 | 0:23:17 | |
'Ordinarily, sound waves will interact with my outer ear | 0:23:17 | 0:23:21 | |
'before travelling inside.' | 0:23:21 | 0:23:23 | |
When I clap, I make a broadband sound, | 0:23:23 | 0:23:25 | |
so it has many sound frequencies in it. | 0:23:25 | 0:23:27 | |
As the sound comes in, depending on where it comes from, | 0:23:27 | 0:23:30 | |
it'll hit different parts of your ear. | 0:23:30 | 0:23:32 | |
As it hits these complicated folds, | 0:23:32 | 0:23:34 | |
some sound frequencies are made louder and others are made quieter, | 0:23:34 | 0:23:38 | |
and your brain's learned over time how to interpret these changes | 0:23:38 | 0:23:41 | |
that occur, according to where the sound comes from. | 0:23:41 | 0:23:44 | |
You're listening for really subtle changes in the frequency composition | 0:23:44 | 0:23:47 | |
of the sound that are introduced by | 0:23:47 | 0:23:49 | |
the folded structure of the outer ear. | 0:23:49 | 0:23:51 | |
So, the ear here is not just guiding sound in, this outer bit, | 0:23:51 | 0:23:55 | |
it's actually changing it. | 0:23:55 | 0:23:57 | |
So, it's really clever. That's really complicated | 0:23:57 | 0:23:59 | |
and really clever at the same time. | 0:23:59 | 0:24:01 | |
It is really clever and you have to learn to do it. | 0:24:01 | 0:24:03 | |
Everyone's ears are different | 0:24:03 | 0:24:05 | |
and the peculiarities of your outer ear are special to you. | 0:24:05 | 0:24:08 | |
'The properties of sound waves and the way they travel | 0:24:10 | 0:24:14 | |
'carry important messages about our environment. | 0:24:14 | 0:24:17 | |
'But once those messages enter our ears, they need to be translated.' | 0:24:22 | 0:24:26 | |
In order to access this information that's all around us, | 0:24:32 | 0:24:35 | |
we need a detector. | 0:24:35 | 0:24:37 | |
Something that can convert these tiny vibrations of the air | 0:24:37 | 0:24:40 | |
into a signal our brain can understand. | 0:24:40 | 0:24:43 | |
'Most of us take hearing for granted, | 0:24:44 | 0:24:47 | |
'because it happens apparently automatically deep inside our ears.' | 0:24:47 | 0:24:51 | |
The reason that we can hear so much and so well | 0:24:52 | 0:24:55 | |
is that our ears are sophisticated detectors - | 0:24:55 | 0:24:58 | |
a series of different structures all working together. | 0:24:58 | 0:25:02 | |
If just one of the links in that chain is broken | 0:25:02 | 0:25:05 | |
the consequences can be devastating. | 0:25:05 | 0:25:07 | |
'I miss not hearing the birds. | 0:25:15 | 0:25:17 | |
'I lost my hearing very, very quickly. | 0:25:20 | 0:25:24 | |
'You can't believe it's happening. | 0:25:24 | 0:25:26 | |
'You think, "Oh, did I hear something?" | 0:25:29 | 0:25:32 | |
'But, no, you don't. | 0:25:33 | 0:25:35 | |
'It really is frightening.' | 0:25:39 | 0:25:42 | |
'This is Barbara. | 0:25:49 | 0:25:51 | |
'She lives with her husband, Tony, | 0:25:51 | 0:25:54 | |
'and they've been married for 53 years.' | 0:25:54 | 0:25:57 | |
LAUGHTER | 0:25:57 | 0:25:59 | |
-What's funny? -Hm? | 0:25:59 | 0:26:01 | |
What's funny? | 0:26:01 | 0:26:02 | |
'But, for the past year and a half, | 0:26:02 | 0:26:05 | |
'they've not been able to communicate properly.' | 0:26:05 | 0:26:07 | |
Crashed on...the wires...! | 0:26:07 | 0:26:10 | |
'Because, very suddenly, Barbara became profoundly deaf.' | 0:26:12 | 0:26:16 | |
'I can't hear anything round out here. | 0:26:18 | 0:26:21 | |
'I just miss my old life in general, really. | 0:26:21 | 0:26:25 | |
'Yeah.' | 0:26:25 | 0:26:26 | |
Not, sort of, hearing people or knowing what they're talking about. | 0:26:26 | 0:26:30 | |
That's quite difficult. | 0:26:30 | 0:26:32 | |
Deafness is a lonely world. | 0:26:32 | 0:26:35 | |
'Barbara lost her hearing because just one small part of her ear | 0:26:36 | 0:26:41 | |
'stopped working. | 0:26:41 | 0:26:43 | |
'When sound enters a healthy ear, it gets funnelled through | 0:26:51 | 0:26:55 | |
'to a coiled up structure called the cochlea - | 0:26:55 | 0:26:58 | |
'a spiral-shaped cavity containing some 16,000 specialised cells | 0:26:58 | 0:27:04 | |
'called hair cells. | 0:27:04 | 0:27:06 | |
'As the sound wave moves through the cochlea, | 0:27:07 | 0:27:10 | |
'the cells' hairlike protrusions are displaced... | 0:27:10 | 0:27:13 | |
'..causing the cell to send electrical impulses | 0:27:15 | 0:27:18 | |
'along nerve fibres that are destined for the brain. | 0:27:18 | 0:27:22 | |
'But Barbara's hair cells are no longer working, | 0:27:26 | 0:27:30 | |
'which means that although the rest of her ear is healthy, | 0:27:30 | 0:27:33 | |
'her brain is completely starved of sound.' | 0:27:33 | 0:27:37 | |
'I miss my independence.' | 0:27:39 | 0:27:41 | |
What I try not to do is get down. I try to think positive. | 0:27:43 | 0:27:48 | |
How are you feeling about today? | 0:27:51 | 0:27:54 | |
-I'm OK. Yeah. -OK? | 0:27:54 | 0:27:56 | |
How about you? | 0:27:56 | 0:27:58 | |
Bit nervous, I suppose. | 0:27:58 | 0:28:01 | |
'A month ago, Barbara was fitted with a cochlear implant. | 0:28:01 | 0:28:05 | |
'An array of electrodes has been threaded into her cochlea | 0:28:05 | 0:28:09 | |
'that will take over the role of her faulty hair cells. | 0:28:09 | 0:28:12 | |
'And today, at Southampton University, | 0:28:12 | 0:28:15 | |
'it will be switched on and tested for the first time. | 0:28:15 | 0:28:18 | |
So, I'm going to switch it on, OK? | 0:28:18 | 0:28:21 | |
-Can you hear anything? -Not yet, no. | 0:28:25 | 0:28:27 | |
Just going to bring it up. | 0:28:27 | 0:28:29 | |
Nothing. | 0:28:30 | 0:28:31 | |
BEEPING | 0:28:39 | 0:28:41 | |
Very faint. | 0:28:41 | 0:28:43 | |
Very, very faint. | 0:28:43 | 0:28:45 | |
BEEPING | 0:28:45 | 0:28:46 | |
Very gradual, isn't it? | 0:28:46 | 0:28:49 | |
Yeah. Bit more? | 0:28:49 | 0:28:50 | |
Yes. | 0:28:50 | 0:28:52 | |
I'm going to keep talking as I bring it up, OK? | 0:28:52 | 0:28:55 | |
Just going to keep bringing it up. | 0:28:55 | 0:28:57 | |
How did you get here today, Tony? | 0:28:57 | 0:28:59 | |
I can hear... Can't understand. | 0:28:59 | 0:29:01 | |
I can almost hear my own voice again! | 0:29:01 | 0:29:04 | |
How's the volume now? | 0:29:06 | 0:29:08 | |
How's the volume? | 0:29:08 | 0:29:11 | |
-Yes! -The volume? | 0:29:11 | 0:29:13 | |
-The volume. How's the volume now, you said, yes. -Yeah. | 0:29:13 | 0:29:17 | |
What can you hear? | 0:29:17 | 0:29:19 | |
-Can you hear me? -Yes, I can hear you. | 0:29:19 | 0:29:22 | |
Oh, dear. | 0:29:22 | 0:29:23 | |
No, it's good. | 0:29:23 | 0:29:26 | |
Yeah. | 0:29:26 | 0:29:28 | |
'For the first time in over a year, | 0:29:29 | 0:29:31 | |
'Barbara's brain is receiving sound signals.' | 0:29:31 | 0:29:35 | |
-OK? -That's amazing. | 0:29:35 | 0:29:37 | |
When you take it off I can hear nothing. | 0:29:37 | 0:29:40 | |
Amazing, yes. | 0:29:42 | 0:29:43 | |
Don't make me cry! | 0:29:43 | 0:29:45 | |
Don't worry about a hanky. | 0:29:48 | 0:29:50 | |
-So, you're noticing the difference? -It's incredible. | 0:29:51 | 0:29:54 | |
Stop it. You're going to make me cry. | 0:29:55 | 0:29:58 | |
Thank you. | 0:29:58 | 0:30:00 | |
Oh, dear. | 0:30:01 | 0:30:03 | |
I didn't think it would be this quick. | 0:30:03 | 0:30:05 | |
No, you're doing really well. | 0:30:05 | 0:30:07 | |
I thought for my birthday in July I might be able to hear then. | 0:30:07 | 0:30:10 | |
What are we going to have for dinner tonight, some champagne? | 0:30:11 | 0:30:15 | |
Stop it. You'll make me cry again! | 0:30:15 | 0:30:18 | |
'Barbara is no longer lost in silence. | 0:30:26 | 0:30:29 | |
'By translating sound into electrical signals, | 0:30:32 | 0:30:35 | |
'the implant replicates the cochlea's key job, | 0:30:35 | 0:30:39 | |
'returning Barbara to a world full of sound. | 0:30:39 | 0:30:43 | |
'The cochlea is a truly extraordinary structure, | 0:30:49 | 0:30:52 | |
'doing much more than simply translating noise. | 0:30:52 | 0:30:56 | |
'It's also able to discriminate the incredible variation of sounds | 0:30:56 | 0:31:00 | |
'in our environment.' | 0:31:00 | 0:31:02 | |
Even though it's quite quiet and calm where I am now, there's still | 0:31:02 | 0:31:05 | |
a huge richness of information in the sound around me. | 0:31:05 | 0:31:08 | |
And a lot of that richness comes in the frequency of the sound, | 0:31:08 | 0:31:12 | |
the number of times every second that air molecules are vibrating | 0:31:12 | 0:31:16 | |
backwards and forwards. | 0:31:16 | 0:31:17 | |
It could be a hundred times or a thousand times | 0:31:17 | 0:31:20 | |
and they're all overlaid on top of each other. | 0:31:20 | 0:31:23 | |
So, the singing birds and the distant road | 0:31:23 | 0:31:25 | |
are all creating an environment | 0:31:25 | 0:31:27 | |
that's full of different frequencies | 0:31:27 | 0:31:29 | |
and that is really useful information. | 0:31:29 | 0:31:31 | |
'Our cochlea has a really clever way of telling us | 0:31:40 | 0:31:43 | |
'which frequencies are coming into the ear. | 0:31:43 | 0:31:46 | |
'It exploits a phenomenon called resonance | 0:31:48 | 0:31:51 | |
'which can be demonstrated with these conkers.' | 0:31:51 | 0:31:54 | |
You can see if I push on one and I push on another one, | 0:31:55 | 0:31:59 | |
this one with the short string is going backwards and forwards | 0:31:59 | 0:32:03 | |
really quite quickly. | 0:32:03 | 0:32:04 | |
Whereas this one down here with a longer string, | 0:32:04 | 0:32:07 | |
you can see it swings much, much more slowly. | 0:32:07 | 0:32:11 | |
Each one has its own natural frequency. | 0:32:11 | 0:32:14 | |
And it's different for every conker | 0:32:14 | 0:32:16 | |
because the string is a different length. | 0:32:16 | 0:32:18 | |
Now, the clever bit comes when a frequency comes from somewhere else. | 0:32:18 | 0:32:22 | |
And I'm going to demonstrate that here with this apple. | 0:32:22 | 0:32:24 | |
If I swing the apple, what happens is that the apple | 0:32:24 | 0:32:27 | |
will gently move the string and that's forcing all the conkers | 0:32:27 | 0:32:31 | |
to oscillate at the same frequency as the apple, | 0:32:31 | 0:32:35 | |
however longer their string is. | 0:32:35 | 0:32:37 | |
And you can see that these ones are moving a little bit, | 0:32:37 | 0:32:40 | |
moving a little bit, little bit more, and this one, | 0:32:40 | 0:32:42 | |
this one is the one that's really responding. | 0:32:42 | 0:32:45 | |
And if you look at it from this angle, you can see that this conker | 0:32:45 | 0:32:49 | |
is the one that's got the same length of string as the apple. | 0:32:49 | 0:32:52 | |
The others are hardly moving at all and this one is swinging loads. | 0:32:52 | 0:32:55 | |
'And I can show you what happens | 0:32:55 | 0:32:57 | |
'when I change the frequency of the driving force. | 0:32:57 | 0:33:00 | |
'By shortening the string, I can make the apple swing faster.' | 0:33:01 | 0:33:05 | |
We can see that this time it's this one. | 0:33:06 | 0:33:09 | |
This conker is responding really, really strongly | 0:33:09 | 0:33:12 | |
and this is the one again that's got more or less | 0:33:12 | 0:33:15 | |
the same length of string as the apple. | 0:33:15 | 0:33:17 | |
It's got the same natural frequency as the oscillation coming in. | 0:33:17 | 0:33:21 | |
And now it's trying to hit me in the face! | 0:33:22 | 0:33:25 | |
This is the phenomenon of resonance. | 0:33:26 | 0:33:29 | |
This is very similar to what's happening in the cochlea. | 0:33:29 | 0:33:32 | |
'Just as the conker strings have a variety of natural frequencies, | 0:33:36 | 0:33:40 | |
'so do structures in the ear. | 0:33:40 | 0:33:43 | |
'The thousands of tiny hair cells that send messages to the brain | 0:33:46 | 0:33:50 | |
'sit along a structure called the basilar membrane. | 0:33:50 | 0:33:54 | |
'This stretched piece of elastic that runs through the cochlea | 0:33:54 | 0:33:57 | |
'has different natural frequencies as you go along it. | 0:33:57 | 0:34:01 | |
It's got one end which is narrow and taut | 0:34:01 | 0:34:04 | |
and it's got a very high natural frequency of oscillation | 0:34:04 | 0:34:07 | |
and the other end of the basilar membrane is wider and less taut | 0:34:07 | 0:34:11 | |
and that's got a lower frequency of oscillation. | 0:34:11 | 0:34:13 | |
So, when sound comes into our ear, | 0:34:14 | 0:34:17 | |
the whole basilar membrane will vibrate a little bit | 0:34:17 | 0:34:20 | |
but one part of it will really start to vibrate. | 0:34:20 | 0:34:23 | |
The one that matches the frequency of the sound coming in. | 0:34:23 | 0:34:27 | |
And it's the hair cells at that part of the basilar membrane | 0:34:28 | 0:34:32 | |
that are stimulated, that send the sound into our brains | 0:34:32 | 0:34:36 | |
and that's how our ears tell us which frequencies of sound | 0:34:36 | 0:34:40 | |
are coming in from the environment around us. | 0:34:40 | 0:34:42 | |
'This elegant and simple mechanism gives us the ability to detect | 0:34:45 | 0:34:50 | |
'and interpret an enormous range of frequencies. | 0:34:50 | 0:34:54 | |
'A far greater range of sounds than the spectrum of light waves | 0:34:54 | 0:34:58 | |
'we can see with our eyes. | 0:34:58 | 0:35:00 | |
'From low-sounding noises that go through 20 cycles a second | 0:35:04 | 0:35:09 | |
'and have wavelengths 17 metres long. | 0:35:09 | 0:35:13 | |
'All the way through to very high-frequency sounds | 0:35:17 | 0:35:20 | |
'that can exceed 18,000 cycles a second | 0:35:20 | 0:35:24 | |
'and have a wavelength of under two centimetres. | 0:35:24 | 0:35:28 | |
'The cochlea's a sophisticated structure | 0:35:31 | 0:35:34 | |
'that lets us detect a huge variety of sounds. | 0:35:34 | 0:35:37 | |
This story is interesting because it passed through | 0:35:37 | 0:35:40 | |
one of the most significant stages in evolutionary history. | 0:35:40 | 0:35:45 | |
When hearing and life first evolved, it all happened underwater. | 0:35:45 | 0:35:50 | |
'Which would mean that, one day, it would have to confront and overcome | 0:35:51 | 0:35:55 | |
'a physical law of nature. | 0:35:55 | 0:35:57 | |
'3.5 billion years ago, life began in the oceans. | 0:36:02 | 0:36:07 | |
'And as organisms became ever more complex, | 0:36:10 | 0:36:13 | |
'they developed increasingly sophisticated senses. | 0:36:13 | 0:36:17 | |
'Around 400 million years ago, | 0:36:22 | 0:36:25 | |
'fish became the first hearing animal, | 0:36:25 | 0:36:28 | |
'evolving structures that, | 0:36:28 | 0:36:30 | |
'although much simpler than the modern cochlea, | 0:36:30 | 0:36:33 | |
'worked in a similar way.' | 0:36:33 | 0:36:35 | |
Ears underwater were fluid-filled cavities | 0:36:37 | 0:36:40 | |
and so sound could easily travel from the water | 0:36:40 | 0:36:44 | |
into the underwater ear and it could easily be detected | 0:36:44 | 0:36:47 | |
because there was liquid on both sides of that boundary. | 0:36:47 | 0:36:50 | |
'But when that life came up into air, | 0:36:52 | 0:36:55 | |
'suddenly the sound was in the air | 0:36:55 | 0:36:57 | |
'but the ear was still filled with fluid | 0:36:57 | 0:36:59 | |
'and that was a problem.' | 0:36:59 | 0:37:01 | |
I've got a set up here that will show what happens when sound | 0:37:07 | 0:37:11 | |
tries to travel across a boundary from air into water. | 0:37:11 | 0:37:15 | |
I've got two microphones here. One's a normal microphone. This one. | 0:37:15 | 0:37:18 | |
It's set up for hearing sound in air. | 0:37:18 | 0:37:21 | |
And the other one is set up for hearing sound underwater, down here. | 0:37:21 | 0:37:25 | |
That's called a hydrophone. | 0:37:25 | 0:37:27 | |
I've got some tent pegs here. | 0:37:27 | 0:37:29 | |
I could hear that quite easily and so could the microphone, | 0:37:29 | 0:37:33 | |
so there's a great big spike on the microphone in air. | 0:37:33 | 0:37:36 | |
But the hydrophone in water heard almost nothing. | 0:37:36 | 0:37:39 | |
What's going on is that at the boundary, | 0:37:39 | 0:37:41 | |
when there's air up here and water down here, | 0:37:41 | 0:37:44 | |
and sound comes from the air and hits that boundary, | 0:37:44 | 0:37:47 | |
because air is less dense and much easier to squash than water, | 0:37:47 | 0:37:51 | |
instead of travelling through, | 0:37:51 | 0:37:53 | |
that sound wave just bounces straight off. | 0:37:53 | 0:37:55 | |
It doesn't get through the boundary. | 0:37:55 | 0:37:58 | |
And this is the problem that early life faced. | 0:38:07 | 0:38:10 | |
If you've got a fluid-filled ear, liquid-filled ear, | 0:38:10 | 0:38:13 | |
it works perfectly underwater because sound can travel | 0:38:13 | 0:38:15 | |
through the water into your liquid-filled ear | 0:38:15 | 0:38:18 | |
and you can hear the sound. But once you put that in air, | 0:38:18 | 0:38:21 | |
the sound comes in from the air | 0:38:21 | 0:38:23 | |
but it hits your ear and bounces straight off. | 0:38:23 | 0:38:26 | |
It can't get in to be detected. | 0:38:26 | 0:38:28 | |
'The way sound behaves at a boundary between two mediums | 0:38:29 | 0:38:33 | |
'hindered the ability of early land-based life to hear properly. | 0:38:33 | 0:38:38 | |
The process of evolution came up with a really elegant solution | 0:38:41 | 0:38:44 | |
to this problem, by moving around some very tiny bones. | 0:38:44 | 0:38:48 | |
And here they are. These are life-size casts of them. | 0:38:48 | 0:38:51 | |
And they're called the malleus, the incus and the stapes. | 0:38:51 | 0:38:54 | |
The ossicles, which means "tiny bones". | 0:38:54 | 0:38:57 | |
And they are the smallest bones in the body. | 0:38:57 | 0:39:00 | |
And two of them were part of the jawbone in our marine ancestors | 0:39:00 | 0:39:03 | |
but they moved into the middle ear and they do something very clever. | 0:39:03 | 0:39:06 | |
By working together, | 0:39:06 | 0:39:08 | |
they help move sound from the outside world into the cochlea. | 0:39:08 | 0:39:12 | |
'The ossicles sit just in front of the cochlea. | 0:39:21 | 0:39:24 | |
'And when sound hits the eardrum, these tiny bones are set in motion. | 0:39:26 | 0:39:31 | |
'Moving efficiently as a set of levers | 0:39:33 | 0:39:36 | |
'between the large eardrum and the tiny stapes. | 0:39:36 | 0:39:40 | |
'This increases the energy that's transferred to the cochlea. | 0:39:43 | 0:39:47 | |
'This sophisticated little mechanism acts as an amplifier | 0:39:51 | 0:39:54 | |
'and it's really efficient.' | 0:39:54 | 0:39:55 | |
What matters is the amount of sound energy | 0:39:55 | 0:39:58 | |
that gets into the fluid inside the cochlea. | 0:39:58 | 0:40:01 | |
And without this, it would be about 1%, | 0:40:01 | 0:40:03 | |
but with a middle ear like this, it's about 60%. | 0:40:03 | 0:40:07 | |
So, this is the crucial evolutionary step | 0:40:07 | 0:40:10 | |
that allowed land-based mammals to develop such good hearing. | 0:40:10 | 0:40:13 | |
'Hearing that allows us to detect a huge range of amplitudes. | 0:40:16 | 0:40:21 | |
'Everything from the thundering roar of an engine... | 0:40:22 | 0:40:25 | |
'..to the flapping of an insect's wings. | 0:40:27 | 0:40:30 | |
'And hearing the very quiet end of this range | 0:40:32 | 0:40:35 | |
'doesn't rely solely on the ear but also on what lies beyond it. | 0:40:35 | 0:40:41 | |
'To experience this, I need to find something extremely rare. | 0:40:45 | 0:40:50 | |
'Silence. | 0:40:50 | 0:40:52 | |
'It doesn't exist in the natural world | 0:40:53 | 0:40:56 | |
'so I've come here - the largest anechoic chamber in Britain. | 0:40:56 | 0:41:00 | |
'It's been meticulously engineered to be incredibly quiet. | 0:41:03 | 0:41:08 | |
'And it's here that I'll test my ears to their limit.' | 0:41:08 | 0:41:12 | |
The idea of all this clobber is that I have to be in there | 0:41:18 | 0:41:21 | |
completely on my own. | 0:41:21 | 0:41:23 | |
So, there's no sources of sound and nothing to reflect off. | 0:41:23 | 0:41:26 | |
So, this might be a moot point because I might decide I hate it | 0:41:29 | 0:41:32 | |
after two minutes and that's all right. | 0:41:32 | 0:41:34 | |
But if I'm all right after 20 minutes, is there any reason to... | 0:41:34 | 0:41:37 | |
Does it get worse as you go? Because some people don't seem to mind it. | 0:41:37 | 0:41:40 | |
I think it's completely individual and so you, kind of, see how it is. | 0:41:40 | 0:41:43 | |
'All on my own, I can feel myself adjusting to this new environment. | 0:42:05 | 0:42:10 | |
'I can't hear any sounds from outside. | 0:42:12 | 0:42:15 | |
'It's the quietest place I've ever been. | 0:42:16 | 0:42:18 | |
'And as I sit, the rustle of my clothes sounds strangely loud. | 0:42:25 | 0:42:31 | |
HEART BEATING | 0:42:32 | 0:42:34 | |
'I'm starting to notice the sounds of my own body. | 0:42:36 | 0:42:39 | |
'The regular beating of my heart. | 0:42:42 | 0:42:44 | |
'A background hiss, perhaps from the firing of my nerves. | 0:42:46 | 0:42:50 | |
'The soft whisper of my breath. | 0:42:54 | 0:42:56 | |
'Sounds that I don't ordinarily hear have now become dominant.' | 0:43:00 | 0:43:04 | |
Oh, they're opening the door. | 0:43:17 | 0:43:20 | |
I wonder what the outside world's going to be like now. | 0:43:20 | 0:43:23 | |
'After 50 minutes, Dr Peter Keating arrives to explain | 0:43:24 | 0:43:28 | |
'how I could hear so much in a place like this. | 0:43:28 | 0:43:32 | |
-So, how was that? -It wasn't ever completely silent. | 0:43:33 | 0:43:36 | |
My brain was always telling me it was hearing something | 0:43:36 | 0:43:38 | |
but that something was very, very quiet. | 0:43:38 | 0:43:40 | |
When you take external sounds away, which is what's happening here, | 0:43:40 | 0:43:43 | |
then first of all you become more sensitive to the sounds | 0:43:43 | 0:43:46 | |
that are inside your body. | 0:43:46 | 0:43:48 | |
There's actually a little separate set of cells in your auditory nerve | 0:43:48 | 0:43:51 | |
which are responsible for hearing very quiet sounds. | 0:43:51 | 0:43:53 | |
So, in here, you were probably switching over to using those. | 0:43:53 | 0:43:56 | |
'A specialised type of nerve fibre | 0:43:59 | 0:44:01 | |
'carries very quiet sound signals from the cochlea to the brain, | 0:44:01 | 0:44:06 | |
'where our sensitivity to this type of sound isn't fixed. | 0:44:06 | 0:44:10 | |
The brain is constantly adapting, | 0:44:11 | 0:44:13 | |
and so, if you take away loud sounds and you only have quiet sounds, | 0:44:13 | 0:44:16 | |
the brain will get used to that over time. | 0:44:16 | 0:44:19 | |
So, the physical hearing apparatus is staying the same | 0:44:19 | 0:44:22 | |
-but our brains are what's doing the adapting? -Absolutely. | 0:44:22 | 0:44:25 | |
So, when you came in here, in the first seconds to minutes, | 0:44:25 | 0:44:28 | |
there would have been some changes going on in your brain. | 0:44:28 | 0:44:31 | |
If you'd stayed in here for longer, if you'd stayed in for days, weeks, | 0:44:31 | 0:44:34 | |
more changes would have happened. | 0:44:34 | 0:44:36 | |
And if you'd stayed in here for months, | 0:44:36 | 0:44:37 | |
even more changes would have happened. | 0:44:37 | 0:44:39 | |
That's one of the things that we're finding out about the brain | 0:44:39 | 0:44:42 | |
is that you can adapt to these changes in sensory input. | 0:44:42 | 0:44:45 | |
Not just hearing, but in vision and all kinds of other sensory systems. | 0:44:45 | 0:44:48 | |
And these can happen at all kinds of different timescales. | 0:44:48 | 0:44:51 | |
'The processing power of our brain, | 0:44:52 | 0:44:54 | |
'together with the mechanics of our ears, | 0:44:54 | 0:44:56 | |
'forms an incredibly powerful and adaptive system | 0:44:56 | 0:44:59 | |
'to listen in to the world. | 0:44:59 | 0:45:01 | |
Understanding the physical properties of sound | 0:45:22 | 0:45:25 | |
and being able to decipher them to learn about the world around us | 0:45:25 | 0:45:29 | |
is a really powerful tool. | 0:45:29 | 0:45:31 | |
But we're not limited to just listening in | 0:45:31 | 0:45:33 | |
on what the environment sends to us. | 0:45:33 | 0:45:36 | |
We can create our own sound to send it out to probe the world. | 0:45:36 | 0:45:41 | |
And that can teach us about ourselves, our planet | 0:45:41 | 0:45:44 | |
and even what's beyond that. | 0:45:44 | 0:45:46 | |
'Sound has been especially useful in looking at things we can't see. | 0:45:56 | 0:46:01 | |
'Things that are hidden from the world of light. | 0:46:01 | 0:46:04 | |
'It began in the early years of the First World War, | 0:46:07 | 0:46:10 | |
'when submarines became a deadly weapon.' | 0:46:10 | 0:46:13 | |
EXPLOSION | 0:46:18 | 0:46:20 | |
'Almost invisible, | 0:46:20 | 0:46:22 | |
'these machines would drive the Allies | 0:46:22 | 0:46:25 | |
'to develop new detection technology. | 0:46:25 | 0:46:27 | |
'Sound can travel exceptionally long distances underwater | 0:46:30 | 0:46:34 | |
'and so acoustic echo ranging, or sonar, offered an obvious solution. | 0:46:34 | 0:46:39 | |
'And after the Second World War had come to an end, | 0:46:42 | 0:46:45 | |
'the rapid advancements of underwater acoustics continued. | 0:46:45 | 0:46:48 | |
'Our relationship with the oceans can be limited. | 0:46:51 | 0:46:54 | |
'Quite often you look out over the sea and what you see is this. | 0:46:54 | 0:46:57 | |
'It's grey and opaque, you can't see through the surface. | 0:46:57 | 0:47:01 | |
'It looks a little bit dull.' | 0:47:01 | 0:47:02 | |
But underwater acoustics changed all of that. | 0:47:03 | 0:47:07 | |
Once you can use sound to explore the underwater world, | 0:47:07 | 0:47:10 | |
you're not limited to looking for submarines. | 0:47:10 | 0:47:13 | |
'Today, even as we reach for the stars, | 0:47:14 | 0:47:17 | |
'we know less about this ocean than we do the surface of the moon.' | 0:47:17 | 0:47:22 | |
'By the 1950s, oceanographers across the world | 0:47:22 | 0:47:25 | |
'were using military sonar technology | 0:47:25 | 0:47:27 | |
'to look down at the deep ocean floor, | 0:47:27 | 0:47:30 | |
'which, for centuries, we could only imagine. | 0:47:30 | 0:47:33 | |
'They discovered an extraordinary underwater landscape | 0:47:36 | 0:47:39 | |
'of towering mountains and deep trenches. | 0:47:39 | 0:47:42 | |
'Sound played a key role in understanding | 0:47:45 | 0:47:48 | |
'the magnificent structures of our world.' | 0:47:48 | 0:47:51 | |
The oceans are one of the most important features of our planet | 0:47:51 | 0:47:55 | |
and they're not just the filler between the interesting bits. | 0:47:55 | 0:47:59 | |
Once you can see them properly, | 0:47:59 | 0:48:01 | |
you can see the oceans become a place. | 0:48:01 | 0:48:04 | |
'Today, sonar doesn't just show us large-scale structures, | 0:48:10 | 0:48:14 | |
'it can also reveal exquisite detail.' | 0:48:14 | 0:48:17 | |
-Welcome aboard. -Thank you. | 0:48:17 | 0:48:19 | |
'Which, until recently, had been a job only our eyes could perform. | 0:48:19 | 0:48:23 | |
'This is the North Sea, off the coast of Suffolk.' | 0:48:24 | 0:48:28 | |
Looks just like an ordinary bit of ocean | 0:48:30 | 0:48:33 | |
but there is an archaeological site down there, | 0:48:33 | 0:48:36 | |
so I'm going down to have a look. | 0:48:36 | 0:48:38 | |
I have a lot of layers to put on. | 0:48:38 | 0:48:41 | |
Oops. The other way round. | 0:48:42 | 0:48:43 | |
Right, it's definitely cold in the North Sea! | 0:48:54 | 0:48:58 | |
-I can't actually see you. -You can't see me at all? | 0:48:58 | 0:49:00 | |
-Unless you come in really close. -So... -Yeah, it's just so brown. | 0:49:00 | 0:49:04 | |
I've got my glove here. | 0:49:04 | 0:49:06 | |
And if I hold that out, in front of my face underwater, you can't | 0:49:06 | 0:49:11 | |
see anything, so I can't see this far in front of my face. | 0:49:11 | 0:49:14 | |
And the reason it's this brown, horrible colour, | 0:49:14 | 0:49:18 | |
is that the water is clearly full of sediment. | 0:49:18 | 0:49:21 | |
There's tiny little particles of silt and sand. | 0:49:21 | 0:49:25 | |
And so seeing anything... | 0:49:25 | 0:49:27 | |
is virtually impossible. | 0:49:27 | 0:49:29 | |
'Even though we're near the coast, | 0:49:33 | 0:49:35 | |
'where the water isn't particularly deep, | 0:49:35 | 0:49:38 | |
'the visibility is still appalling.' | 0:49:38 | 0:49:40 | |
That's... That's terrifying. | 0:49:44 | 0:49:46 | |
I was only going down a metre or two and it's completely black. | 0:49:47 | 0:49:52 | |
Like, absolutely dark. | 0:49:52 | 0:49:54 | |
'Since I couldn't see anything for myself, | 0:49:54 | 0:49:57 | |
'Professor David Sear explains what lies beneath us.' | 0:49:57 | 0:50:01 | |
When I was down there a little while ago I couldn't see anything. | 0:50:01 | 0:50:05 | |
So, what is down there? | 0:50:05 | 0:50:06 | |
Well, actually, down there is one of the largest archaeological sites | 0:50:06 | 0:50:10 | |
in the world, called Dunwich. | 0:50:10 | 0:50:12 | |
Dunwich, to a lot of people, is just a small village. | 0:50:12 | 0:50:15 | |
800 years ago it was the sixth largest international port | 0:50:15 | 0:50:18 | |
in the North Sea. | 0:50:18 | 0:50:19 | |
And the story of Dunwich is one of coastal erosion. | 0:50:19 | 0:50:23 | |
Coastal erosion driven by a series of very large storms. | 0:50:23 | 0:50:27 | |
So, this sounds like the perfect job for sonar. | 0:50:27 | 0:50:29 | |
What do you see when you look with sonar? | 0:50:29 | 0:50:31 | |
Sonar enabled us to cover a large area | 0:50:31 | 0:50:34 | |
and we were able to see that there were indeed structures. | 0:50:34 | 0:50:37 | |
The important thing was that we didn't know whether they were | 0:50:37 | 0:50:40 | |
geology or were they actually parts of churches and buildings? | 0:50:40 | 0:50:44 | |
So, what you ideally need is a technology | 0:50:44 | 0:50:46 | |
that is able to see through this turbid, muddy water | 0:50:46 | 0:50:49 | |
with the detail to enable you to see individual, | 0:50:49 | 0:50:53 | |
say, carved blocks or other evidence of it being made by people. | 0:50:53 | 0:50:57 | |
We came across a technology that is relatively new | 0:50:57 | 0:51:00 | |
and it does just that. | 0:51:00 | 0:51:02 | |
It uses sound to project... A bit like a torch beam, but sound. | 0:51:04 | 0:51:08 | |
And you don't do that from a boat? | 0:51:08 | 0:51:11 | |
You don't. You have to send a diver down | 0:51:11 | 0:51:13 | |
and that diver sees what the sound is illuminating, if you like, | 0:51:13 | 0:51:18 | |
in their visor. | 0:51:18 | 0:51:19 | |
'Sound waves from surface-based sonar | 0:51:26 | 0:51:28 | |
'can travel easily through the water, which provided David | 0:51:28 | 0:51:32 | |
'with the layout and general structure | 0:51:32 | 0:51:34 | |
'of this two-kilometre-squared site. | 0:51:34 | 0:51:37 | |
'Yet it was the much higher frequency sound waves | 0:51:38 | 0:51:41 | |
'from the sonar camera that gave David what he really needed. | 0:51:41 | 0:51:45 | |
'Although these sound waves can't travel as far, | 0:51:45 | 0:51:48 | |
'they can create much more detailed images, | 0:51:48 | 0:51:51 | |
'and showed that what lay beneath the waves | 0:51:51 | 0:51:54 | |
'were structures with sharp straight edges. | 0:51:54 | 0:51:57 | |
'Edges that could only have been made by man.' | 0:51:58 | 0:52:01 | |
The first time we saw this imagery, | 0:52:09 | 0:52:11 | |
looking at it in real-time as the diver saw it, it was fantastic, | 0:52:11 | 0:52:15 | |
because you could see great blocks of masonry, made of flints, | 0:52:15 | 0:52:19 | |
rubble, mortar, just like the churches today on land. | 0:52:19 | 0:52:23 | |
You see it on the seabed. | 0:52:23 | 0:52:25 | |
That nailed it for us. It was the evidence we needed | 0:52:25 | 0:52:28 | |
to move from the historical accounts to the reality of, | 0:52:28 | 0:52:31 | |
yes, these are the ruins of churches from medieval Dunwich. | 0:52:31 | 0:52:34 | |
'Sending sound waves through the ocean | 0:52:34 | 0:52:37 | |
'has unlocked marine archaeology, | 0:52:37 | 0:52:39 | |
'uncovering the human stories hidden beneath the sea. | 0:52:39 | 0:52:43 | |
'We're continually getting better at detecting and controlling | 0:52:50 | 0:52:54 | |
'the nuances of sound waves | 0:52:54 | 0:52:56 | |
'and at using them as tools for probing and manipulating our world. | 0:52:56 | 0:53:00 | |
'But there are other worlds out there. | 0:53:01 | 0:53:04 | |
'Even though sound can't travel across the solar system, | 0:53:05 | 0:53:08 | |
'every planet and moon is like a little bubble of sound | 0:53:08 | 0:53:12 | |
'isolated from us by the vacuum of space. | 0:53:12 | 0:53:15 | |
'And there's a huge amount to learn from those little bubbles of sound, | 0:53:16 | 0:53:19 | |
'if only we can listen in.' | 0:53:19 | 0:53:22 | |
'Three, two, one... | 0:53:23 | 0:53:26 | |
'And lift-off of the Cassini spacecraft!' | 0:53:26 | 0:53:29 | |
'In 1997, one of the largest spacecraft ever launched | 0:53:29 | 0:53:33 | |
'started its billion-kilometre journey.' | 0:53:33 | 0:53:37 | |
'We have cleared the tower | 0:53:38 | 0:53:40 | |
'and the Cassini spacecraft is on its way to Saturn.' | 0:53:40 | 0:53:43 | |
'In 2005, Cassini sent a probe called Huygens to Titan, | 0:53:47 | 0:53:52 | |
'the largest of Saturn's moons, | 0:53:52 | 0:53:55 | |
'A world shrouded by a thick, opaque atmosphere... | 0:53:59 | 0:54:04 | |
'..making it almost impossible to explore from a distance. | 0:54:06 | 0:54:10 | |
'So, for decades, this moon remained much of a mystery.' | 0:54:13 | 0:54:17 | |
Huygens is still the only probe to have successfully landed | 0:54:20 | 0:54:24 | |
in the outer solar system. | 0:54:24 | 0:54:26 | |
And as it deployed its parachutes and started this two-and-a-half-hour | 0:54:26 | 0:54:30 | |
drift down through the atmosphere of Titan towards the surface, | 0:54:30 | 0:54:34 | |
there was a suite of instruments on the probe measuring all sorts | 0:54:34 | 0:54:38 | |
of things about the environment and the conditions. | 0:54:38 | 0:54:40 | |
And some of those instruments were recording sound. | 0:54:40 | 0:54:44 | |
'Around 160km above the surface of Titan, | 0:54:46 | 0:54:51 | |
'Huygens deployed a microphone, | 0:54:51 | 0:54:54 | |
'which recorded the sounds of Titan's atmosphere. | 0:54:54 | 0:54:57 | |
SOUND OF STRONG WIND | 0:55:02 | 0:55:05 | |
And this is it. | 0:55:11 | 0:55:12 | |
This is what the microphone on Huygens heard | 0:55:12 | 0:55:15 | |
as it fell through Titan's atmosphere. | 0:55:15 | 0:55:18 | |
What you're hearing is the roaring of the wind going past the probe | 0:55:19 | 0:55:23 | |
and the probe falling down through the atmosphere. | 0:55:23 | 0:55:27 | |
This is the sound of an alien world, and this was only the start. | 0:55:27 | 0:55:32 | |
'Another instrument used sonar to detect the surface | 0:55:37 | 0:55:41 | |
'during the final 90 metres of the descent. | 0:55:41 | 0:55:44 | |
'It showed that Titan's terrain rises and falls. | 0:55:44 | 0:55:48 | |
'That the surface is relatively smooth, | 0:55:49 | 0:55:52 | |
'not dissimilar to gravel, | 0:55:52 | 0:55:54 | |
'and that this surface is likely to be damp.' | 0:55:54 | 0:55:57 | |
This is the landscape that Huygens landed on. | 0:55:58 | 0:56:01 | |
Sonar was one of the tools that helps us understand it. | 0:56:01 | 0:56:05 | |
Even if a planet or a moon hasn't got an atmosphere, | 0:56:06 | 0:56:09 | |
sound can still be generated and transmitted through its liquid | 0:56:09 | 0:56:12 | |
and solid layers, so potentially, if you sent an acoustic probe | 0:56:12 | 0:56:16 | |
to another world, you might hear the sound of thunder, | 0:56:16 | 0:56:20 | |
or hear meteorite strikes, | 0:56:20 | 0:56:22 | |
or the flow of rivers. Perhaps rivers of methane. | 0:56:22 | 0:56:25 | |
Or the sound of rain. | 0:56:25 | 0:56:27 | |
And as more and more missions are sent out into the solar system | 0:56:27 | 0:56:30 | |
to explore, acoustic probes are going to become more and more common | 0:56:30 | 0:56:34 | |
as a way of exploring not just our world but others. | 0:56:34 | 0:56:38 | |
'We live in a dynamic, pulsating world of sound | 0:57:03 | 0:57:07 | |
'and it touches our skin and our clothes and our lives every day.' | 0:57:07 | 0:57:10 | |
We can only tap into it because we have these two complex, | 0:57:10 | 0:57:14 | |
sensitive detectors on either side of our head, | 0:57:14 | 0:57:17 | |
but that's enough to sense the riches. | 0:57:17 | 0:57:20 | |
Sound is so important for our species. | 0:57:26 | 0:57:28 | |
It's deeply embedded in our culture | 0:57:28 | 0:57:31 | |
and it's allowing us to push our technological boundaries | 0:57:31 | 0:57:34 | |
to better understand our world. | 0:57:34 | 0:57:36 | |
And the best thing about it is that that world of sound is right here. | 0:57:36 | 0:57:40 | |
All you have to do is listen. | 0:57:40 | 0:57:42 | |
FIREWORKS | 0:57:42 | 0:57:45 | |
If you'd like to find out more about the science of sound | 0:57:52 | 0:57:55 | |
and how we hear sound, go to the BBC website on screen | 0:57:55 | 0:57:59 | |
and follow the links to the Open University. | 0:57:59 | 0:58:02 |