Browse content similar to The Problem Solvers. Check below for episodes and series from the same categories and more!
Line | From | To | |
---|---|---|---|
For centuries, we thought animals were creatures of pure instinct, | 0:00:05 | 0:00:10 | |
with no understanding of how the world works. | 0:00:10 | 0:00:13 | |
Simple beasts with simple minds. | 0:00:15 | 0:00:18 | |
We thought that only we humans could actually think, | 0:00:20 | 0:00:23 | |
analyse, reason, use imagination and solve problems. | 0:00:23 | 0:00:28 | |
But new scientific research is revealing there is | 0:00:31 | 0:00:34 | |
a small group of animals with extraordinarily clever minds - | 0:00:34 | 0:00:38 | |
octopus, great apes | 0:00:38 | 0:00:42 | |
and even some birds. | 0:00:42 | 0:00:45 | |
It's a bizarrely diverse group. | 0:00:45 | 0:00:47 | |
But they share a remarkable ability to analyse | 0:00:48 | 0:00:52 | |
and solve problems in a very human-like way. | 0:00:52 | 0:00:55 | |
I want to get inside these animals' minds | 0:00:55 | 0:00:58 | |
and see just how clever they are. | 0:00:58 | 0:01:00 | |
Starting with perhaps the cleverest of all - the crow family. | 0:01:03 | 0:01:08 | |
How on earth did that crow do that? | 0:01:08 | 0:01:12 | |
I want to push them to the limit, to find out how | 0:01:14 | 0:01:17 | |
they measure up to other animals that we think are pretty clever. | 0:01:17 | 0:01:22 | |
All you've got to do is lift the ball! | 0:01:22 | 0:01:25 | |
I'll discover what makes some animals capable of brilliant | 0:01:25 | 0:01:29 | |
ingenuity that seems almost human. | 0:01:29 | 0:01:33 | |
It's a sort of Satnav for bees. | 0:01:33 | 0:01:35 | |
I want to uncover why this special group of animals | 0:01:37 | 0:01:41 | |
have such superb problem solving minds. | 0:01:41 | 0:01:44 | |
Now, if we can do this, we might just unlock one of nature's | 0:01:45 | 0:01:49 | |
greatest mysteries - how we ourselves came to be so clever. | 0:01:49 | 0:01:54 | |
Welcome to a lost world. | 0:02:12 | 0:02:15 | |
The island of New Caledonia. | 0:02:18 | 0:02:21 | |
Lying nearly 1,000 miles east of Australia, it is a tropical | 0:02:26 | 0:02:30 | |
paradise, bursting with exotic life found nowhere else on the planet. | 0:02:30 | 0:02:37 | |
I'm here to find one of these unique animals. | 0:02:41 | 0:02:46 | |
It has an almost legendary status, | 0:02:46 | 0:02:49 | |
thanks to an amazing ability to solve problems. | 0:02:49 | 0:02:53 | |
The animal that I am looking for is truly remarkable. | 0:02:55 | 0:02:59 | |
The problem is, it is very, very difficult to find. | 0:02:59 | 0:03:03 | |
It's cunning. And it's quick. | 0:03:03 | 0:03:05 | |
Agile. | 0:03:05 | 0:03:07 | |
If I'm honest with you, there could be one right here somewhere, | 0:03:08 | 0:03:11 | |
just looking down at me, and I've never ever seen one. | 0:03:11 | 0:03:15 | |
Many experts think that these are the cleverest animals on earth. | 0:03:23 | 0:03:28 | |
I've been told I might see that intelligence in action | 0:03:28 | 0:03:32 | |
on the island's high mountain roads. | 0:03:32 | 0:03:34 | |
It is this - a crow. | 0:03:37 | 0:03:41 | |
But not just any crow. | 0:03:41 | 0:03:44 | |
This is the New Caledonian crow. | 0:03:44 | 0:03:47 | |
It is not the same species we find a Britain. | 0:03:47 | 0:03:51 | |
These are jungle birds. | 0:03:51 | 0:03:53 | |
And here, in isolation on the island, | 0:03:53 | 0:03:56 | |
they have evolved some remarkable abilities. | 0:03:56 | 0:03:59 | |
This one is holding a nut in its claw. | 0:03:59 | 0:04:03 | |
It drops it onto the tarmac, | 0:04:04 | 0:04:06 | |
seemingly to crack open the hard shell. | 0:04:06 | 0:04:09 | |
But in truth, it is what the bird does next that is really ingenious. | 0:04:10 | 0:04:16 | |
Now, look at this, this is interesting because, | 0:04:16 | 0:04:18 | |
having broken the nut open on the road, | 0:04:18 | 0:04:21 | |
these birds are then carrying it to the Armco here, | 0:04:21 | 0:04:25 | |
where there are small depressions, man-made cuts, in it. | 0:04:25 | 0:04:29 | |
And they're using these as a vice to stop the nut from rolling | 0:04:29 | 0:04:33 | |
around whilst they access the fruit on the inside of it. | 0:04:33 | 0:04:37 | |
Now, that strikes me as pretty clever. | 0:04:37 | 0:04:41 | |
But the question is, of course, just how clever are these birds? | 0:04:41 | 0:04:45 | |
To find out, I have come to visit Dr Alex Taylor | 0:04:49 | 0:04:52 | |
at his field aviary here on the island. | 0:04:52 | 0:04:55 | |
To study how these birds solve problems, | 0:04:58 | 0:05:00 | |
he has put a tasty morsel of food deep in a container. | 0:05:00 | 0:05:04 | |
He is testing whether they can work out how to reach it | 0:05:04 | 0:05:08 | |
using a variety of objects he has pleased in the aviary. | 0:05:08 | 0:05:12 | |
But first, he is trying his puzzle out on me. | 0:05:12 | 0:05:16 | |
Imagine that you are a crow. | 0:05:16 | 0:05:19 | |
Here is your food, in a deep hole. How would you go about, | 0:05:19 | 0:05:22 | |
with the tools available to you on this table, | 0:05:22 | 0:05:24 | |
solving this problem? | 0:05:24 | 0:05:25 | |
-This is part of it, obviously. -Mm-hm. | 0:05:28 | 0:05:30 | |
At my disposal, I have a short stick on a piece of string, | 0:05:30 | 0:05:36 | |
three stones inside the cages... | 0:05:36 | 0:05:39 | |
..and a longer stick trapped in a box. | 0:05:40 | 0:05:43 | |
Which means, then, that the crow is going to use this stick... | 0:05:43 | 0:05:48 | |
-..to get the stones out of there. -Absolutely. | 0:05:51 | 0:05:55 | |
Next, the crow needs to drop the stones onto a trap door | 0:05:55 | 0:05:58 | |
to release the long stick. | 0:05:58 | 0:06:01 | |
It is going to need all three stones. | 0:06:01 | 0:06:03 | |
And then it probably will drop that one out. Excellent. | 0:06:03 | 0:06:08 | |
And using this long stick, it will finally be able to reach the food. | 0:06:08 | 0:06:13 | |
This is a tough one. All right, can I stay in here? | 0:06:15 | 0:06:18 | |
-Absolutely, you can sit and watch. We will see what happens. -Come on, then. | 0:06:18 | 0:06:21 | |
Send in your mastermind, because it is going to need that. | 0:06:21 | 0:06:24 | |
Alex studies wild birds, | 0:06:26 | 0:06:28 | |
which he releases after three months of research. | 0:06:28 | 0:06:31 | |
This one is nicknamed 007, | 0:06:33 | 0:06:36 | |
and it is about to attempt what Alex believes is one of the most | 0:06:36 | 0:06:40 | |
complex tests of the animal mind ever constructed. | 0:06:40 | 0:06:45 | |
The bird is familiar with the individual objects, | 0:06:45 | 0:06:49 | |
but this is the first time he has seen them arranged like this. | 0:06:49 | 0:06:54 | |
Eight separate stages that must be completed in a specific order | 0:06:56 | 0:07:01 | |
if the puzzle is to be solved. | 0:07:01 | 0:07:03 | |
And if the bird succeeds, it will be a world first. | 0:07:03 | 0:07:08 | |
He takes time to have a look and then starts with the short stick. | 0:07:08 | 0:07:14 | |
Stage I. | 0:07:19 | 0:07:20 | |
He finds it is too short to reach the food. | 0:07:25 | 0:07:28 | |
He then sets off to get the first stone. | 0:07:29 | 0:07:32 | |
But he drops it. | 0:07:40 | 0:07:42 | |
And another. He seems to be stuck. | 0:07:56 | 0:07:59 | |
But then something seems to click. | 0:08:03 | 0:08:07 | |
He deploys the first stone. | 0:08:08 | 0:08:10 | |
And then another. | 0:08:24 | 0:08:26 | |
Got it! | 0:08:44 | 0:08:46 | |
The eighth and final stage. | 0:08:48 | 0:08:50 | |
Success! | 0:08:57 | 0:08:59 | |
Eight individual stages of one complex puzzle completed. | 0:08:59 | 0:09:04 | |
That was remarkable. | 0:09:07 | 0:09:10 | |
I've never ever seen anything like it. | 0:09:10 | 0:09:14 | |
Of all of the bird behaviour I've seen, nothing matches that. | 0:09:14 | 0:09:19 | |
I can hardly believe it. | 0:09:19 | 0:09:20 | |
I'm still just running that sequence through my mind. | 0:09:20 | 0:09:23 | |
It happened really quickly. | 0:09:23 | 0:09:25 | |
But the immediate question is, of course, | 0:09:25 | 0:09:28 | |
how on earth did that crow do that? | 0:09:28 | 0:09:32 | |
You see, on the face of it, | 0:09:34 | 0:09:36 | |
the crow's problem-solving abilities seem mind boggling. | 0:09:36 | 0:09:40 | |
But look close enough and the natural world is filled with | 0:09:44 | 0:09:48 | |
examples of animals behaving in clever ways. | 0:09:48 | 0:09:51 | |
The spider spins a web that gets stronger when it catches prey. | 0:09:56 | 0:10:01 | |
It's precision engineered to catch flies. | 0:10:01 | 0:10:05 | |
Turtles navigate through thousands of miles of featureless ocean, | 0:10:07 | 0:10:12 | |
returning every year to the same beaches to lay their eggs. | 0:10:12 | 0:10:16 | |
It is as if they hold a nautical map in their heads. | 0:10:18 | 0:10:22 | |
So are the clever crows really so unusual? | 0:10:24 | 0:10:27 | |
Well, to find out, | 0:10:30 | 0:10:32 | |
I need to investigate how animals solve problems. | 0:10:32 | 0:10:37 | |
And I am starting back in Britain with the honeybee, | 0:10:40 | 0:10:43 | |
a small animal that seems to be able to do something staggeringly clever. | 0:10:43 | 0:10:48 | |
The pollen and nectar that bees eat | 0:10:50 | 0:10:52 | |
is clearly only available whilst the flowers are in bloom, | 0:10:52 | 0:10:56 | |
so it's there throughout the spring and the summer. | 0:10:56 | 0:10:58 | |
But the bees have to get enough of it | 0:10:58 | 0:11:01 | |
so that the hive can make it through the winter. | 0:11:01 | 0:11:04 | |
And frankly, if that isn't a massive task, then I don't know what is. | 0:11:04 | 0:11:08 | |
But the bees have an almost incredible solution. | 0:11:11 | 0:11:15 | |
To see with that is, I have come to meet Professor Adam Hart. | 0:11:15 | 0:11:19 | |
-Hello, Adam. -Hi, Chris. | 0:11:20 | 0:11:22 | |
One of his specialisms is the hidden world of the honeybee. | 0:11:22 | 0:11:26 | |
That is a very intimate view of the interior of the hive. | 0:11:26 | 0:11:29 | |
-Yes, it's like Big Brother for bees. -It is! It's great, isn't it, really? | 0:11:29 | 0:11:32 | |
Adam has set up an infrared camera to spy on the bees inside the hive. | 0:11:34 | 0:11:38 | |
And we are hoping to see a striking behaviour that is key to | 0:11:40 | 0:11:45 | |
solving the problem of gathering enough food. | 0:11:45 | 0:11:48 | |
It is called the waggle dance. | 0:11:48 | 0:11:51 | |
Adam, look at this one. This one is waggle dancing, isn't it? | 0:11:53 | 0:11:56 | |
Yeah, this is the waggle dance. | 0:11:56 | 0:11:57 | |
You have got this lovely figure of eight dance, | 0:11:57 | 0:12:00 | |
sometimes getting in the way of other bees, but generally speaking, | 0:12:00 | 0:12:03 | |
keeping this nice kind of rhythm going. | 0:12:03 | 0:12:05 | |
The waggle dance isn't just for show. | 0:12:08 | 0:12:12 | |
This bee has found a good source of food, and she is performing | 0:12:12 | 0:12:17 | |
a set of very precise movements to tell the others | 0:12:17 | 0:12:20 | |
exactly where to find it. | 0:12:20 | 0:12:22 | |
And this is all about communicating the whereabouts of a food source. | 0:12:24 | 0:12:29 | |
Yeah, this is like GPS for bees, basically. | 0:12:29 | 0:12:31 | |
They are telling other bees not just that there is food out there, | 0:12:31 | 0:12:34 | |
which is quite a useful signal, but where that food is. | 0:12:34 | 0:12:36 | |
So they can communicate the direction and the distance of that nectar. | 0:12:36 | 0:12:40 | |
But how can a dance communicate where a food source is? | 0:12:43 | 0:12:48 | |
To find out, we need to get a better view of the horizon. | 0:12:54 | 0:12:59 | |
-Super view, well worth the climb. -Yeah. | 0:13:04 | 0:13:06 | |
But what about the mechanics of this dance, then? | 0:13:06 | 0:13:08 | |
The bee we saw in the hive was doing this figure of eight, | 0:13:08 | 0:13:11 | |
and that central section was where she was vibrating her abdomen. | 0:13:11 | 0:13:13 | |
When you think about it, the only frame of reference bees have | 0:13:13 | 0:13:16 | |
in the hive is gravity, up and down. | 0:13:16 | 0:13:17 | |
The frame of reference they have out here is the position of the sun, | 0:13:17 | 0:13:20 | |
or more accurately, where the sun would be if it was projected down | 0:13:20 | 0:13:23 | |
on the horizon, what is called the azimuth. | 0:13:23 | 0:13:26 | |
From up here, it is easy to see where the sun lies over | 0:13:27 | 0:13:30 | |
the landscape and where it would be if it dropped to the horizon. | 0:13:30 | 0:13:36 | |
Now, our dancing bee was waggling at an angle of five degrees. | 0:13:36 | 0:13:42 | |
So, if this tower were a massive beehive, | 0:13:42 | 0:13:45 | |
the waggle dance would be telling us that the nectar is five degrees | 0:13:45 | 0:13:50 | |
from the relative position of the sun. | 0:13:50 | 0:13:52 | |
That is somewhere in that direction. | 0:13:52 | 0:13:55 | |
All right, that is the direction, what about the distance? | 0:13:57 | 0:14:00 | |
That's that duration of that central waggle run, as it is called. | 0:14:00 | 0:14:03 | |
The longer they dance for, the longer that waggle run goes on for, | 0:14:03 | 0:14:05 | |
the farther away the food is. | 0:14:05 | 0:14:07 | |
So, a very short waggle run, like we saw down in the hive, | 0:14:07 | 0:14:09 | |
is maybe a couple of kilometres, whereas they can sometimes | 0:14:09 | 0:14:12 | |
waggle for much longer, perhaps nine, ten kilometres even. | 0:14:12 | 0:14:16 | |
So, honeybees survive the winter | 0:14:17 | 0:14:20 | |
by using what we'd call mathematics. | 0:14:20 | 0:14:24 | |
They compute angles and distances | 0:14:24 | 0:14:26 | |
and then communicate that information to their nest mates. | 0:14:26 | 0:14:30 | |
It seems incredibly clever, but in fact, | 0:14:34 | 0:14:38 | |
it doesn't involve any thought at all. | 0:14:38 | 0:14:41 | |
You see, the waggle dance is so important to the survival | 0:14:41 | 0:14:46 | |
of the hive, that it comes built into the brain of the honeybee. | 0:14:46 | 0:14:50 | |
Every worker bee is born | 0:14:50 | 0:14:52 | |
with the ability to perform the dance automatically. | 0:14:52 | 0:14:57 | |
In other words, the bees are acting on instinct - | 0:14:58 | 0:15:03 | |
a set of behaviours | 0:15:03 | 0:15:04 | |
that are so important to the animal's survival | 0:15:04 | 0:15:07 | |
that they are hard wired. | 0:15:07 | 0:15:10 | |
Think of them as a simple set of rules that are tattooed | 0:15:10 | 0:15:14 | |
onto its genes by evolution. | 0:15:14 | 0:15:17 | |
It is what makes these bees do the waggle dance. | 0:15:17 | 0:15:20 | |
The animals have no real understanding | 0:15:20 | 0:15:23 | |
of what they are doing. | 0:15:23 | 0:15:25 | |
But what about our New Caledonian crow? | 0:15:27 | 0:15:30 | |
It solved a complex eight-stage problem. | 0:15:30 | 0:15:36 | |
Is it really possible that the bird was acting only on instinct, | 0:15:36 | 0:15:41 | |
without any real understanding of what it was doing? | 0:15:41 | 0:15:45 | |
In short, the answer is no. | 0:15:45 | 0:15:49 | |
Because, unlike bees with their waggle dance, these birds are not | 0:15:49 | 0:15:53 | |
dependent on solving a multistage puzzle in order to survive, | 0:15:53 | 0:15:59 | |
for the very simple reason that you don't find multistage | 0:15:59 | 0:16:02 | |
puzzles like this out there in the wild. They are not natural. | 0:16:02 | 0:16:06 | |
They are a man-made artefact. | 0:16:06 | 0:16:09 | |
And for that very obvious reason, there is no chance whatsoever | 0:16:09 | 0:16:14 | |
that these birds could have evolved an instinct to solve them. | 0:16:14 | 0:16:19 | |
But solve them they do. So if it isn't instinct, what is it? | 0:16:19 | 0:16:23 | |
To begin to discover what is really going on in the crow's mind, | 0:16:25 | 0:16:29 | |
I have come to Somerset to meet Lloyd Buck. | 0:16:29 | 0:16:32 | |
He handles birds for TV and film. | 0:16:32 | 0:16:35 | |
And this is Bran. | 0:16:36 | 0:16:38 | |
He is a raven, one of the largest members of the crow family, | 0:16:38 | 0:16:43 | |
a group known as the corvids. | 0:16:43 | 0:16:45 | |
Oh, what a stunner! | 0:16:48 | 0:16:51 | |
Hold my finger. | 0:16:54 | 0:16:56 | |
Are you going to do it again? | 0:16:56 | 0:16:57 | |
No, he just wants... Oh, ups, sorry. | 0:16:57 | 0:16:59 | |
You want to hold my finger? No. | 0:17:01 | 0:17:04 | |
You like that camera, it's the highest point. | 0:17:04 | 0:17:07 | |
Bran is clearly a bird who knows his own mind. | 0:17:07 | 0:17:11 | |
In fact, it is hard to know who is in charge. | 0:17:11 | 0:17:14 | |
Myself and my dogs, you know, I am the pack leader. | 0:17:16 | 0:17:19 | |
What about you and Bran, who's boss? | 0:17:19 | 0:17:21 | |
Oh, well, I think obviously Ban. | 0:17:21 | 0:17:24 | |
But as far as he is concerned, I am his mate. | 0:17:24 | 0:17:26 | |
-Right. His mate? -Yeah. -His partner? -His partner. | 0:17:26 | 0:17:29 | |
It is going to be a long-term relationship | 0:17:29 | 0:17:31 | |
because they're a long-lived bird, aren't they? | 0:17:31 | 0:17:33 | |
Yeah, in captivity up to 40, 50, even 60 years. | 0:17:33 | 0:17:38 | |
-He is going the distance with you. -Yeah. | 0:17:38 | 0:17:39 | |
And they are really time intensive. | 0:17:39 | 0:17:42 | |
You can't just forget him and leave him in his aviary. | 0:17:42 | 0:17:45 | |
He likes to go out for his fly, his walk every day. | 0:17:45 | 0:17:48 | |
He wants to see, he wants to spend time with you. | 0:17:48 | 0:17:50 | |
Cos they are so intelligent. | 0:17:50 | 0:17:52 | |
To keep his demanding bird occupied, | 0:17:53 | 0:17:56 | |
Lloyd likes to set Bran difficult problems to solve, | 0:17:56 | 0:17:59 | |
like this one. | 0:17:59 | 0:18:01 | |
Lloyd places a piece of food inside a plastic bottle | 0:18:02 | 0:18:06 | |
and then crushes it. | 0:18:06 | 0:18:09 | |
-And you give him a bowl of water. -A bowl of water? -Yes. | 0:18:09 | 0:18:13 | |
With the bottle crushed, the food is trapped behind the restriction. | 0:18:17 | 0:18:21 | |
But this doesn't stop Bran. | 0:18:21 | 0:18:24 | |
First, he adds water. | 0:18:27 | 0:18:29 | |
Next, he spins it, | 0:18:32 | 0:18:34 | |
and the liquid carries the food past the restriction and out. | 0:18:34 | 0:18:38 | |
That is a clever piece of problem-solving. | 0:18:40 | 0:18:43 | |
Oh, he's got it. He's got it! | 0:18:47 | 0:18:51 | |
Honestly! | 0:18:51 | 0:18:53 | |
What about that? Now, that is impressive. | 0:18:53 | 0:18:57 | |
The thing is, Lloyd, how long did it take him to work that out? | 0:18:57 | 0:19:00 | |
Well, presented with the crushed bottle, | 0:19:00 | 0:19:02 | |
it took him basically an afternoon. | 0:19:02 | 0:19:04 | |
He got frustrated, he couldn't work it out initially, and he left it. | 0:19:04 | 0:19:08 | |
So we left it with him in his aviary | 0:19:08 | 0:19:09 | |
and we went up to the house for lunch. | 0:19:09 | 0:19:11 | |
Come back down in the afternoon and, lo and behold, the food was gone. | 0:19:11 | 0:19:16 | |
But let's not forget that Bran is a tame raven. | 0:19:20 | 0:19:24 | |
Aren't you a clever boy? Yes, you are. | 0:19:24 | 0:19:28 | |
It is possible that, in spending so much time with humans, | 0:19:28 | 0:19:31 | |
he has been able to observe and copy some of their actions. | 0:19:31 | 0:19:35 | |
You are too clever for your own good half the time, that is your problem. | 0:19:36 | 0:19:40 | |
So, I'm going to set Bran a new challenge, | 0:19:40 | 0:19:43 | |
one that he has never seen before. | 0:19:43 | 0:19:46 | |
And this time, | 0:19:46 | 0:19:48 | |
I am going to pit his intelligence against another animal, | 0:19:48 | 0:19:52 | |
one that we tend to think is pretty clever, | 0:19:52 | 0:19:54 | |
certainly brighter than birds - | 0:19:54 | 0:19:57 | |
the dog, | 0:19:57 | 0:19:58 | |
or to be more precise, my dogs - Itchy and Scratchy. | 0:19:58 | 0:20:03 | |
-Amongst the dogs, poodles are about the smartest. -Are they? -So they say. | 0:20:03 | 0:20:08 | |
Bran, he just finished War And Peace. | 0:20:08 | 0:20:11 | |
-LAUGHTER -I can see how this is going to go. | 0:20:11 | 0:20:13 | |
To make things even more interesting, | 0:20:16 | 0:20:18 | |
I am also going to put the same test to Fletcher here, | 0:20:18 | 0:20:21 | |
who is two and a half, | 0:20:21 | 0:20:23 | |
which incidentally is nearly the same age as Bran. | 0:20:23 | 0:20:26 | |
They are all going to face this - a puzzle box of my own design. | 0:20:29 | 0:20:34 | |
Here's how it works. | 0:20:34 | 0:20:36 | |
The prize is in the green ball in the centre. | 0:20:38 | 0:20:41 | |
But to get to it, they firstly have to pull this drop door down here. | 0:20:41 | 0:20:45 | |
And then, pull this smaller box out. | 0:20:45 | 0:20:50 | |
And then, remove the lid from that | 0:20:50 | 0:20:55 | |
to get the ball. | 0:20:55 | 0:20:56 | |
Simple, isn't it? | 0:20:56 | 0:20:58 | |
Well, let's find out. | 0:20:58 | 0:21:00 | |
We've given all of our contenders | 0:21:01 | 0:21:03 | |
the chance to familiarise themselves with parts of the puzzle box. | 0:21:03 | 0:21:07 | |
As usual, Lloyd leaves Bran to it. | 0:21:11 | 0:21:14 | |
Fletcher is also showing an interest in how it works. | 0:21:16 | 0:21:19 | |
Itch, concentrate. | 0:21:21 | 0:21:22 | |
'Whereas I need to be a bit more hands-on.' | 0:21:22 | 0:21:26 | |
Now, look, look. | 0:21:26 | 0:21:27 | |
Pull that. What this. Look, watch. | 0:21:29 | 0:21:31 | |
This is the sort of thing my mother would have done. Look, watch. | 0:21:31 | 0:21:35 | |
That's your lesson over for today. | 0:21:38 | 0:21:40 | |
And now, the moment of truth has come. | 0:21:42 | 0:21:44 | |
The contenders are about to face the test proper for the first time. | 0:21:44 | 0:21:49 | |
This is raven versus dog versus human. | 0:21:49 | 0:21:54 | |
Itchy, solve this. | 0:22:02 | 0:22:04 | |
Solve that. OK? I'll be back. Solve it. | 0:22:07 | 0:22:09 | |
And that, I'm afraid to say, is an emphatic win for Bran | 0:22:14 | 0:22:19 | |
and all of his corvid kind. | 0:22:19 | 0:22:21 | |
In truth, my dogs didn't even seem to realise there was | 0:22:24 | 0:22:27 | |
a problem to solve, despite all of my training efforts. | 0:22:27 | 0:22:31 | |
And Fletcher, well, he played around with the box a bit, | 0:22:32 | 0:22:36 | |
but soon seemed to lose interest. | 0:22:36 | 0:22:38 | |
Whilst Bran was so quick that we will have to use a high-speed | 0:22:38 | 0:22:43 | |
camera just to see how he did it. | 0:22:43 | 0:22:46 | |
So the question is, | 0:22:49 | 0:22:50 | |
why are ravens like Bran able to solve problems on their own when | 0:22:50 | 0:22:55 | |
other species, including clever dogs and even young humans, can't. | 0:22:55 | 0:23:01 | |
To find out what abilities the corvids have | 0:23:14 | 0:23:16 | |
that other animals seem to lack, I have come to Cambridge | 0:23:16 | 0:23:20 | |
to meet Professor Nikki Clayton. | 0:23:20 | 0:23:22 | |
She is a world expert on the crow family, | 0:23:22 | 0:23:25 | |
which includes these Eurasian jays. | 0:23:25 | 0:23:29 | |
She sets up experiments to break down the different abilities | 0:23:29 | 0:23:33 | |
that these birds use to solve problems. | 0:23:33 | 0:23:36 | |
And she set one of them, Hoy, a fiendish challenge. | 0:23:36 | 0:23:40 | |
One that wouldn't be out of place in a physics lesson. | 0:23:41 | 0:23:44 | |
She has dropped some wax worms, his favourite food, | 0:23:45 | 0:23:49 | |
into a tube of water, out of reach. | 0:23:49 | 0:23:51 | |
So the bird needs to work out how to raise the water level. | 0:23:51 | 0:23:56 | |
Nikki, let's see what's going on, then. | 0:23:59 | 0:24:02 | |
-Fire up the laptop and see what he is up to. -I will do. | 0:24:02 | 0:24:04 | |
Nikki's birds have minimal human contact, | 0:24:05 | 0:24:08 | |
so we're watching Hoy from a safe distance. | 0:24:08 | 0:24:11 | |
Straight away, he starts dropping stones into the water. | 0:24:12 | 0:24:17 | |
They are quite specific | 0:24:17 | 0:24:19 | |
about what they use, actually, | 0:24:19 | 0:24:20 | |
and how many stones. | 0:24:20 | 0:24:22 | |
They don't put more stones in than they need. | 0:24:22 | 0:24:24 | |
-He is checking the water rise every time he puts the stone in. -Yeah. | 0:24:24 | 0:24:29 | |
He pops up and you can see the eye looking down. Look at that. | 0:24:29 | 0:24:33 | |
-Look at that, the small stone wasn't going to be enough. -No. | 0:24:34 | 0:24:37 | |
There we go, go on. | 0:24:39 | 0:24:41 | |
No, can't quite reach. | 0:24:43 | 0:24:45 | |
You know, Nikki, it is almost | 0:24:45 | 0:24:47 | |
as if he understands the effect of dropping that stone in. | 0:24:47 | 0:24:51 | |
That's right. He only does this when there is liquid in the tube. | 0:24:51 | 0:24:55 | |
If there is a worm in the tube and it's filled with sand, | 0:24:55 | 0:24:58 | |
he doesn't bother. He knows that it needs to be a liquid | 0:24:58 | 0:25:01 | |
in that tube in order for the stones to work. | 0:25:01 | 0:25:04 | |
Hoy understands that a sinking stone will cause the water level | 0:25:06 | 0:25:11 | |
to rise, and this in turn will allow him to reach the wax worm. | 0:25:11 | 0:25:16 | |
He also knows the same technique won't work with sand. | 0:25:16 | 0:25:20 | |
And this reveals the first skill animals need to solve problems - | 0:25:20 | 0:25:26 | |
the ability to understand the rules of cause and effect. | 0:25:26 | 0:25:32 | |
But I am intrigued to know | 0:25:34 | 0:25:36 | |
how Hoy worked all of this out in his mind. | 0:25:36 | 0:25:40 | |
He was trained that, if he drops or knocks a stone | 0:25:41 | 0:25:45 | |
into a tube, he gets a worm. | 0:25:45 | 0:25:48 | |
But in the training apparatus, the worm comes out the bottom, | 0:25:48 | 0:25:50 | |
not out the top, and there is no water. | 0:25:50 | 0:25:52 | |
And having learned to associate stones with tubes to get food, | 0:25:52 | 0:25:57 | |
what he has been able to do is then to transfer this to a novel problem, | 0:25:57 | 0:26:02 | |
where there is water, | 0:26:02 | 0:26:03 | |
where the worm comes out a totally different place. | 0:26:03 | 0:26:06 | |
But he has been able to use his information flexibly | 0:26:06 | 0:26:09 | |
and transfer it to novel problems. | 0:26:09 | 0:26:10 | |
-He can join up his knowledge. -Exactly. | 0:26:10 | 0:26:13 | |
So, Hoy was able to solve this problem, | 0:26:16 | 0:26:19 | |
because he already knew how to use the stones. | 0:26:19 | 0:26:23 | |
He is able to learn rules for one situation | 0:26:23 | 0:26:26 | |
and then apply those rules to a new scenario. | 0:26:26 | 0:26:30 | |
Scientists call this flexible thinking, | 0:26:33 | 0:26:37 | |
and it is the second skill that animals need to solve problems. | 0:26:37 | 0:26:42 | |
It is how the New Caledonian crow solved the multistage problem | 0:26:42 | 0:26:48 | |
and also how Bran made such short work of my puzzle box. | 0:26:48 | 0:26:54 | |
The birds were thinking flexibly, | 0:26:56 | 0:26:59 | |
using previous experience to solve new problems. | 0:26:59 | 0:27:03 | |
And it is an ability that seems sadly missing in my dogs. | 0:27:05 | 0:27:10 | |
Lift the ball, please! Lift it! Lift it! | 0:27:10 | 0:27:14 | |
Is that why Bran, the raven, so convincingly trounced my dogs? | 0:27:15 | 0:27:20 | |
It's exactly that, yeah. | 0:27:20 | 0:27:22 | |
So, the dogs can learn to do something, | 0:27:22 | 0:27:24 | |
but what they can't do is to transfer it to a novel problem. | 0:27:24 | 0:27:28 | |
Yes, I will be gracious in defeat on this account, | 0:27:28 | 0:27:32 | |
given the quality of the opposition. | 0:27:32 | 0:27:34 | |
I suspect, if you had been competing against a pigeon, | 0:27:34 | 0:27:37 | |
you'd have been all right. | 0:27:37 | 0:27:38 | |
They would've even that, that would've been fine. | 0:27:38 | 0:27:41 | |
So, why are some animals better at solving problems than others? | 0:27:48 | 0:27:51 | |
It is something we don't yet fully understand. | 0:27:53 | 0:27:56 | |
But one thing that we do know is that the answer lies | 0:27:56 | 0:27:59 | |
somewhere in here - | 0:27:59 | 0:28:01 | |
the animal brain itself. | 0:28:01 | 0:28:05 | |
Here are a couple of very interesting specimens. | 0:28:05 | 0:28:09 | |
This one is the brain of a dog, in this case a terrier. | 0:28:09 | 0:28:13 | |
And this one here is the brain of a crow. | 0:28:13 | 0:28:17 | |
And it is immediately apparent that the dog's brain is about twice | 0:28:17 | 0:28:21 | |
the size of that of the bird. | 0:28:21 | 0:28:23 | |
So, we might imagine, simplistically, therefore, | 0:28:23 | 0:28:26 | |
that the dog is a more intelligent animal. | 0:28:26 | 0:28:29 | |
But we already know that the crow | 0:28:29 | 0:28:32 | |
can solve problems that the dog can't. | 0:28:32 | 0:28:35 | |
So, clearly, there is | 0:28:35 | 0:28:37 | |
more to cleverness than just the size of the brain. | 0:28:37 | 0:28:41 | |
To investigate what that could be, I have gathered together | 0:28:45 | 0:28:49 | |
a range of preserved animal brains. | 0:28:49 | 0:28:52 | |
If I arrange them in order of body size, a pattern emerges. | 0:28:53 | 0:28:57 | |
The bigger the animal, the bigger the brain. | 0:28:59 | 0:29:03 | |
It seems that the more body you have, the more brain cells | 0:29:06 | 0:29:10 | |
you need to control it. | 0:29:10 | 0:29:12 | |
In other words, there is | 0:29:16 | 0:29:17 | |
a relationship between the mass of the body and the mass of the brain. | 0:29:17 | 0:29:22 | |
And roughly speaking, it is a straight line. | 0:29:26 | 0:29:29 | |
The dog lies pretty much bang on the line here, which is | 0:29:31 | 0:29:35 | |
where you'd expect it, given its body size. | 0:29:35 | 0:29:38 | |
And in fact, most animals lie very close to the line. | 0:29:38 | 0:29:42 | |
But some sit above the line, like we humans. | 0:29:42 | 0:29:47 | |
Our brains are very large for our body size. | 0:29:47 | 0:29:51 | |
But what about the crow? | 0:29:51 | 0:29:54 | |
The crow's brain is also above the line. It is up here, | 0:29:54 | 0:29:58 | |
which means that its brain is bigger | 0:29:58 | 0:30:01 | |
than we would expect just given its body size. | 0:30:01 | 0:30:04 | |
In fact, it is about twice the size. | 0:30:04 | 0:30:07 | |
Now, bizarre as it may seem, | 0:30:07 | 0:30:09 | |
the dog's brain is physically twice the size of the crow's, | 0:30:09 | 0:30:12 | |
but relative to its body size, the crow's brain is bigger than the dog. | 0:30:12 | 0:30:17 | |
So, perhaps this extra mental power will allow the crow to | 0:30:17 | 0:30:23 | |
think in a more complicated and more flexible way than the dog. | 0:30:23 | 0:30:28 | |
And the crow isn't alone in having a brain twice as big as we'd expect. | 0:30:30 | 0:30:36 | |
In fact, it is in the company of another animal | 0:30:36 | 0:30:40 | |
known not only for its big brain but also its cleverness. | 0:30:40 | 0:30:45 | |
It is our nearest relative - the chimpanzee. | 0:30:48 | 0:30:51 | |
And the chimpanzee holds a special place in the history of science. | 0:30:54 | 0:30:58 | |
In the 1960s, a young British scientist, Jane Goodall, | 0:31:00 | 0:31:04 | |
observed chimps doing something that no-one thought animals capable of - | 0:31:04 | 0:31:09 | |
using tools. | 0:31:09 | 0:31:11 | |
I saw this dark shape hunched over a termite mound. | 0:31:13 | 0:31:17 | |
He's making arm movements as though he's sliding it | 0:31:17 | 0:31:20 | |
across the ground and obviously eating. | 0:31:20 | 0:31:23 | |
But that was all I saw. | 0:31:23 | 0:31:25 | |
And I went up to the heap and there were | 0:31:25 | 0:31:27 | |
the pieces of grass lying there, termites moving about the surface. | 0:31:27 | 0:31:31 | |
So I picked up one of these abandoned tools | 0:31:31 | 0:31:34 | |
and pushed it into the mound, and the termites bit on. | 0:31:34 | 0:31:37 | |
It was pretty obvious. | 0:31:37 | 0:31:39 | |
Goodall had observed the chimps using blades of grass as tools | 0:31:45 | 0:31:50 | |
to fish for termites. | 0:31:50 | 0:31:52 | |
It was a discovery of immense significance, | 0:31:53 | 0:31:57 | |
as her PhD supervisor immediately realised. | 0:31:57 | 0:32:00 | |
And he sent his famous reply, "Now we have to redefine man, | 0:32:01 | 0:32:05 | |
"redefine tool or accept chimpanzees as humans." | 0:32:05 | 0:32:09 | |
Goodall's discovery shattered our ideas about what sets us | 0:32:12 | 0:32:16 | |
apart from the rest of the animal kingdom. | 0:32:16 | 0:32:20 | |
We had to accept that animals were cleverer | 0:32:20 | 0:32:22 | |
than we'd given them credit for. | 0:32:22 | 0:32:24 | |
It also gave researchers new insights into how | 0:32:27 | 0:32:30 | |
our ancient ancestors might themselves have solved problems. | 0:32:30 | 0:32:35 | |
And as we have discovered, | 0:32:36 | 0:32:38 | |
chimps are not alone in being able to use tools. | 0:32:38 | 0:32:42 | |
It is something that crows can do, too. | 0:32:42 | 0:32:45 | |
To me, what is so important about using tools is that it reveals | 0:32:58 | 0:33:02 | |
the next crucial ability that an animal needs to solve problems. | 0:33:02 | 0:33:07 | |
For example, take this stick. | 0:33:11 | 0:33:14 | |
If I want to use this as a tool, | 0:33:14 | 0:33:16 | |
I need to be able to see it for more than what it is. | 0:33:16 | 0:33:19 | |
More than just a piece of wood, what a piece of wood might be. | 0:33:19 | 0:33:24 | |
What might it be? | 0:33:24 | 0:33:26 | |
Well, I could sharpen one end and I could perhaps use it as a spear. | 0:33:26 | 0:33:30 | |
Or I could set fire to it to generate some heat. | 0:33:30 | 0:33:33 | |
When I'm going through these thought processes, | 0:33:33 | 0:33:35 | |
there is no doubt that I am using flexible thinking, | 0:33:35 | 0:33:38 | |
understanding the rules of cause-and-effect, | 0:33:38 | 0:33:41 | |
but I am also using a type of thinking which is innovative. | 0:33:41 | 0:33:45 | |
What I am using is imagination. | 0:33:45 | 0:33:49 | |
I'm not just seeing the world as it is now, | 0:33:49 | 0:33:52 | |
I am seeing the world how it could be. | 0:33:52 | 0:33:56 | |
And this raises a very profound question. | 0:33:56 | 0:34:00 | |
Could any other species of animal have an attribute | 0:34:00 | 0:34:04 | |
so significantly human as imagination? | 0:34:04 | 0:34:08 | |
To answer this question, I have come to Austria to meet a rather | 0:34:14 | 0:34:18 | |
inquisitive and endearing type of bird. | 0:34:18 | 0:34:20 | |
And this time it is not a member of the crow family. | 0:34:22 | 0:34:26 | |
These are Goffins cockatoos, a type of parrot. | 0:34:26 | 0:34:30 | |
Big-brained birds with a very curious nature. | 0:34:32 | 0:34:36 | |
Can I have that back? Thank you. | 0:34:40 | 0:34:42 | |
And I am here to meet Dr Alice Auersperg, | 0:34:44 | 0:34:47 | |
an expert in these animals. | 0:34:47 | 0:34:49 | |
She is studying their ability to innovate. | 0:34:51 | 0:34:54 | |
And I am intrigued to know what that might reveal | 0:34:54 | 0:34:57 | |
about their powers of imagination. | 0:34:57 | 0:35:00 | |
-Alice, these birds are very keen to get to know me, it seems. -Yes. | 0:35:01 | 0:35:06 | |
They are especially interested in you because you are new. | 0:35:06 | 0:35:09 | |
And it is not just me that is new. | 0:35:11 | 0:35:14 | |
So is the entire crew. | 0:35:14 | 0:35:16 | |
And everything that we are wearing. | 0:35:20 | 0:35:22 | |
My watch, in particular, has caught the attention of Olympia here. | 0:35:24 | 0:35:29 | |
And within minutes, | 0:35:29 | 0:35:31 | |
she has worked out how to release the clasp. | 0:35:31 | 0:35:33 | |
When they find you, a human, inside the aviary, | 0:35:33 | 0:35:36 | |
they look for everything that looks different on your body, | 0:35:36 | 0:35:40 | |
like your shirt buttons or the shoe laces or your watch. | 0:35:40 | 0:35:43 | |
They go specifically for that | 0:35:43 | 0:35:45 | |
and they stay with it for a very long time. | 0:35:45 | 0:35:48 | |
You like my shiny watch, don't you? | 0:35:48 | 0:35:51 | |
Alice, I think I've had enough of being pecked and probed | 0:35:51 | 0:35:55 | |
and pulled by your rather wonderful cockatoos. | 0:35:55 | 0:35:57 | |
I'd like to see them in action now, mental action. | 0:35:57 | 0:36:00 | |
Look, the watch is off again. | 0:36:00 | 0:36:03 | |
Come on, I know you can steal my watch, but what more can you do? | 0:36:03 | 0:36:06 | |
To investigate what is going on in the minds of these parrots, | 0:36:08 | 0:36:12 | |
Alice created this - the lockbox. | 0:36:12 | 0:36:17 | |
Trapped inside is a tasty nut, | 0:36:17 | 0:36:19 | |
held securely behind this elaborate locking mechanism. | 0:36:19 | 0:36:23 | |
To see how it opens, | 0:36:25 | 0:36:26 | |
we need to employ the services of a master safe-cracker. | 0:36:26 | 0:36:31 | |
Or Muppet, as he is perhaps inappropriately called. | 0:36:31 | 0:36:34 | |
Before we begin, though, there is the question of eyewear. | 0:36:34 | 0:36:39 | |
If you want to start, we have to put sunglasses on | 0:36:40 | 0:36:43 | |
because we could cue the birds with our eye movements. | 0:36:43 | 0:36:46 | |
Can they see where we are looking? Do they have the ability to do that? | 0:36:46 | 0:36:49 | |
We don't know whether this species can, | 0:36:49 | 0:36:51 | |
but it has been proven that some can follow the eye movement of humans. | 0:36:51 | 0:36:55 | |
Better safe than sorry. | 0:36:55 | 0:36:57 | |
We'll forgive fashion for that and make scientific progress. | 0:36:57 | 0:37:01 | |
And here comes Muppet. He's out. | 0:37:01 | 0:37:04 | |
-Oh, he is displaying towards you. -A little bit of display. | 0:37:07 | 0:37:09 | |
Yes, he's a boy now. | 0:37:09 | 0:37:11 | |
Very nice, yes. That is a crest of some distinction. | 0:37:13 | 0:37:15 | |
I had one like that in the 1980s myself. | 0:37:15 | 0:37:18 | |
But perhaps, Muppet, you could take your attention to the lock? | 0:37:18 | 0:37:21 | |
Muppet has done this before, | 0:37:23 | 0:37:26 | |
and he delivers...a masterclass. | 0:37:26 | 0:37:30 | |
He quickly removes the pin and then the screw. | 0:37:30 | 0:37:34 | |
He discards the central bolt before shifting the locking wheel. | 0:37:40 | 0:37:44 | |
And this releases the final bolt. | 0:37:48 | 0:37:51 | |
Voila! | 0:37:51 | 0:37:52 | |
He has reached the nut inside. | 0:37:52 | 0:37:54 | |
Wow. I should have timed it, shouldn't I? | 0:37:59 | 0:38:02 | |
But to make sure that Muppet can't learn the sequence by heart, | 0:38:04 | 0:38:08 | |
Alice can swap the lock sections around or even remove them entirely. | 0:38:08 | 0:38:13 | |
So now I want to change the way in which the box works | 0:38:13 | 0:38:18 | |
and set Muppet a new challenge, one that he has never seen before. | 0:38:18 | 0:38:24 | |
There are five parts. Why don't we take out the middle bit? | 0:38:25 | 0:38:28 | |
-Take out the bolt. -Yes, let's. | 0:38:28 | 0:38:31 | |
The upper section is now redundant, | 0:38:31 | 0:38:34 | |
leaving only the lower parts in operation. | 0:38:34 | 0:38:38 | |
It may look like we have made it easier, but in fact, | 0:38:38 | 0:38:41 | |
we have created an entirely new problem with a different solution. | 0:38:41 | 0:38:46 | |
If Muppet can't see this, he will just repeat what he did before, | 0:38:46 | 0:38:51 | |
and robotically go for the pin at the top. | 0:38:51 | 0:38:54 | |
But if he can see the new problem | 0:38:54 | 0:38:57 | |
and imagine a new solution, | 0:38:57 | 0:39:00 | |
then he will go straight for the wheel. | 0:39:00 | 0:39:03 | |
OK, Alice, let's... let's give him a go. | 0:39:03 | 0:39:07 | |
So now, the moment of truth. | 0:39:07 | 0:39:10 | |
He has gone straight for the wheel. | 0:39:15 | 0:39:18 | |
And then the bar. | 0:39:21 | 0:39:23 | |
And he's in in less than ten seconds. | 0:39:25 | 0:39:28 | |
Muppet got it right first time. | 0:39:30 | 0:39:34 | |
And that gives us a crucial insight into his mind. | 0:39:34 | 0:39:38 | |
He must have looked at the problem, worked it out in his head | 0:39:38 | 0:39:44 | |
and imagined the solution. | 0:39:44 | 0:39:48 | |
That was a hard-earned nut. | 0:39:49 | 0:39:52 | |
So imagination is the third ability that animals need to solve problems. | 0:39:58 | 0:40:05 | |
It's a skill that seems to allow them to work out | 0:40:05 | 0:40:08 | |
new solutions in their heads before putting them into practice. | 0:40:08 | 0:40:13 | |
Up until now, all of the animals that we've looked at | 0:40:20 | 0:40:24 | |
have used their imagination to solve problems | 0:40:24 | 0:40:26 | |
which are sat right in front of them | 0:40:26 | 0:40:29 | |
but we humans can do so much more than that. | 0:40:29 | 0:40:31 | |
We use our imagination to project into the future, | 0:40:31 | 0:40:35 | |
to see problems coming | 0:40:35 | 0:40:37 | |
and think about how we are going to solve them. | 0:40:37 | 0:40:40 | |
Until now, we've always thought of that as a purely human attribute | 0:40:40 | 0:40:45 | |
but what I want to know is can these clever animals do that too? | 0:40:45 | 0:40:49 | |
There is a common behaviour in the animal world | 0:40:54 | 0:40:56 | |
that seems to be about planning for the future. | 0:40:56 | 0:40:59 | |
It's called caching. | 0:40:59 | 0:41:02 | |
It's what squirrels do in the autumn, | 0:41:02 | 0:41:04 | |
hiding nuts in the ground so they can be dug up | 0:41:04 | 0:41:07 | |
and eaten over the winter months. | 0:41:07 | 0:41:09 | |
But if caching were an Olympic sport | 0:41:15 | 0:41:18 | |
then the corvids would be the gold medallists. | 0:41:18 | 0:41:21 | |
Most of them seem to do it, much to the annoyance of the squirrels. | 0:41:21 | 0:41:26 | |
And this American corvid, the Clark's nutcracker, | 0:41:34 | 0:41:37 | |
is the caching king. | 0:41:37 | 0:41:39 | |
Researchers have observed how every year it can store | 0:41:40 | 0:41:44 | |
and remember the location of thousands of different seeds. | 0:41:44 | 0:41:49 | |
This bird truly is the master of memory. | 0:41:53 | 0:41:56 | |
But is this just another example | 0:42:05 | 0:42:07 | |
of instinctive behaviour to survive the winter? | 0:42:07 | 0:42:10 | |
Or is there something far more complex going on? | 0:42:10 | 0:42:15 | |
It's a question that's greatly intrigued Professor Nicky Clayton. | 0:42:15 | 0:42:19 | |
She studies Western scrub-jays, | 0:42:21 | 0:42:23 | |
another American corvid renowned for its caching behaviour. | 0:42:23 | 0:42:28 | |
Nicky wanted to discover whether they could do more | 0:42:28 | 0:42:32 | |
than just remember where they buried food in the past. | 0:42:32 | 0:42:36 | |
If they can travel back in their mind's eye to think about the past, | 0:42:37 | 0:42:42 | |
can they also travel forward in the mind's eye to think about the future? | 0:42:42 | 0:42:46 | |
Can they imagine the future, if you like? Can they plan ahead? | 0:42:46 | 0:42:50 | |
To find out, Nicky created an experiment | 0:42:55 | 0:42:57 | |
based on a very human annoyance - | 0:42:57 | 0:43:00 | |
waking up to find that breakfast is off the menu. | 0:43:00 | 0:43:04 | |
For six days, the birds were housed in this aviary, | 0:43:05 | 0:43:08 | |
split into three zones. | 0:43:08 | 0:43:11 | |
In the middle is the dining room, | 0:43:11 | 0:43:14 | |
where the birds were fed during the day, | 0:43:14 | 0:43:16 | |
and at either end are the bedrooms, where they were kept at night. | 0:43:16 | 0:43:21 | |
But there is a twist. | 0:43:21 | 0:43:23 | |
Kept overnight in this bedroom, | 0:43:23 | 0:43:26 | |
the birds were served an early breakfast | 0:43:26 | 0:43:29 | |
but kept overnight in this one, | 0:43:29 | 0:43:32 | |
they got no breakfast and went hungry until mid-morning. | 0:43:32 | 0:43:36 | |
The birds experienced this daily routine for almost a week. | 0:43:39 | 0:43:43 | |
We give them three experiences of waking up in the hungry room | 0:43:45 | 0:43:49 | |
and three experiences of waking up in the room that serves breakfast. | 0:43:49 | 0:43:53 | |
But the important point is the birds themselves | 0:43:53 | 0:43:56 | |
didn't know which room they would end up in on any given day. | 0:43:56 | 0:43:59 | |
But then Nicky changed the test. | 0:44:03 | 0:44:06 | |
She allowed the birds to cache food in the evening before bedtime. | 0:44:06 | 0:44:11 | |
She placed caching trays in both the hungry and breakfast rooms. | 0:44:13 | 0:44:18 | |
The question was where would they choose to store the food? | 0:44:18 | 0:44:21 | |
Nicky wanted to know if the birds could use their past experience | 0:44:23 | 0:44:27 | |
of the two different rooms and plan for the future, | 0:44:27 | 0:44:32 | |
namely for breakfast time tomorrow. | 0:44:32 | 0:44:34 | |
The results left no doubt. | 0:44:36 | 0:44:38 | |
What we found is that the birds cache about five times | 0:44:41 | 0:44:44 | |
as much in the hungry room as they cache in the breakfast room. | 0:44:44 | 0:44:49 | |
They can imagine what they are going to need the following morning when | 0:44:50 | 0:44:53 | |
they wake up hungry so they can solve a problem before it's even happened. | 0:44:53 | 0:44:58 | |
So what this experiment shows is that the birds can plan for the future. | 0:44:59 | 0:45:04 | |
So the jays' caching behaviour is far more than mere instinct. | 0:45:04 | 0:45:09 | |
They have a grasp of the past, | 0:45:09 | 0:45:12 | |
but can also anticipate future needs and, crucially, plan for it. | 0:45:12 | 0:45:17 | |
This skill is very rare in the animal kingdom | 0:45:17 | 0:45:21 | |
and it's the fourth key ability needed to solve problems. | 0:45:21 | 0:45:26 | |
It's called mental time travel. | 0:45:26 | 0:45:28 | |
It's the ability to go backwards | 0:45:30 | 0:45:32 | |
and forwards in the mind's eye, so it's about projecting yourself | 0:45:32 | 0:45:36 | |
in time to remember the past and to imagine the future. | 0:45:36 | 0:45:41 | |
In humans, mental time travel is a skill that takes a while to develop. | 0:45:46 | 0:45:50 | |
We're not born with it. | 0:45:52 | 0:45:54 | |
These children are about to undergo the sweetie challenge. | 0:45:54 | 0:45:59 | |
The task is simple. | 0:45:59 | 0:46:01 | |
Each child is given a sweet. | 0:46:01 | 0:46:03 | |
They are told that, if they leave it uneaten, | 0:46:03 | 0:46:06 | |
they will get a second one 15 minutes later. | 0:46:06 | 0:46:09 | |
The question is, can they plan for a future in which they have two sweets | 0:46:10 | 0:46:16 | |
or will the lure of instant gratification be too much? | 0:46:16 | 0:46:21 | |
It's a skill that children acquire as they get older. | 0:46:21 | 0:46:25 | |
I think this kind of cognitive capacity is highly sophisticated. | 0:46:27 | 0:46:31 | |
We know that young children don't start developing | 0:46:31 | 0:46:34 | |
these kind of skills until they are at least four years of age. | 0:46:34 | 0:46:38 | |
Clearly, if you are a bird or a human, | 0:46:40 | 0:46:43 | |
it's no bad thing to be a mental time traveller. | 0:46:43 | 0:46:46 | |
When we look at the world's cleverest creatures, | 0:46:54 | 0:46:56 | |
we see a group of very different animals - | 0:46:56 | 0:47:00 | |
the great apes, the corvids, the parrots. | 0:47:00 | 0:47:03 | |
And yet, they all think a little bit like we do. | 0:47:03 | 0:47:06 | |
They have the ability to understand cause and effect | 0:47:07 | 0:47:11 | |
and can utilise this understanding in new and novel situations. | 0:47:11 | 0:47:16 | |
They can also implement imagination and this allows them | 0:47:16 | 0:47:21 | |
to think ahead, to plan in the future. | 0:47:21 | 0:47:24 | |
Now, together, these abilities allow them | 0:47:24 | 0:47:28 | |
something which is incredibly rare in the animal world. | 0:47:28 | 0:47:33 | |
It's the power to reason, the power to solve problems. | 0:47:33 | 0:47:37 | |
But of course, it also raises another question - | 0:47:38 | 0:47:41 | |
what is so special about this group of animals? | 0:47:41 | 0:47:44 | |
What could they possibly have in common? | 0:47:44 | 0:47:47 | |
To answer that, I want to look in a very different environment. | 0:47:52 | 0:47:56 | |
'Here on the island of Bimini in the Bahamas, | 0:48:04 | 0:48:07 | |
'I'm seeking out an elusive creature.' | 0:48:07 | 0:48:09 | |
It's a creature that's as different from a bird or a chimp | 0:48:12 | 0:48:16 | |
as it's possible to be. | 0:48:16 | 0:48:19 | |
Now, this beautiful animal is a common octopus. | 0:48:21 | 0:48:25 | |
Don't worry too much about it being out of water. | 0:48:25 | 0:48:28 | |
They will frequently move over land, between rock pools like these, | 0:48:28 | 0:48:32 | |
so he will be OK for a while. | 0:48:32 | 0:48:34 | |
It lives in a world which is very alien to us - | 0:48:34 | 0:48:38 | |
rock pools and rocky caverns on the coast here. | 0:48:38 | 0:48:41 | |
And as a consequence, its anatomy is very different. | 0:48:41 | 0:48:44 | |
Inside this animal, there are three hearts | 0:48:44 | 0:48:47 | |
but perhaps the most profound difference of all is its brain. | 0:48:47 | 0:48:50 | |
The octopus brain is spread around the body. | 0:48:58 | 0:49:00 | |
Each leg even has its own mini brain to control it. | 0:49:05 | 0:49:09 | |
It's nothing like the one-stop shop that we have. | 0:49:10 | 0:49:13 | |
But what's extraordinary is this animal has problem-solving abilities | 0:49:15 | 0:49:19 | |
similar to those of the great apes... | 0:49:19 | 0:49:22 | |
..the parrots and the crows. | 0:49:24 | 0:49:27 | |
This is a veined octopus just off the coast of Indonesia. | 0:49:31 | 0:49:35 | |
In this environment, it is very exposed to predators | 0:49:35 | 0:49:39 | |
but it has worked out a solution. | 0:49:39 | 0:49:41 | |
It's found a discarded coconut shell | 0:49:44 | 0:49:47 | |
and despite being an unfamiliar object, | 0:49:47 | 0:49:50 | |
the octopus sees its potential. | 0:49:50 | 0:49:52 | |
It tries it on for size before picking it up and carrying it away. | 0:49:52 | 0:49:58 | |
Now, when a threat appears, it has a place to hide | 0:49:58 | 0:50:02 | |
and it can retreat inside its coconut sanctuary. | 0:50:02 | 0:50:05 | |
This is an example of an octopus using tools. | 0:50:06 | 0:50:10 | |
It seems to be using all the key abilities we've seen | 0:50:11 | 0:50:15 | |
in the cleverest animals on land. | 0:50:15 | 0:50:19 | |
So what can the octopus, with its strange brain, | 0:50:19 | 0:50:23 | |
possibly have in common with apes and birds? | 0:50:23 | 0:50:27 | |
When animals are very different, | 0:50:27 | 0:50:29 | |
then the similarities can actually be a lot easier to see | 0:50:29 | 0:50:33 | |
and we have to look beyond the fact that birds can fly, | 0:50:33 | 0:50:37 | |
apes have dextrous hands | 0:50:37 | 0:50:40 | |
or that octopus actually move on their eight arms. | 0:50:40 | 0:50:43 | |
We have to not concentrate so much | 0:50:43 | 0:50:44 | |
on the physical nature of these animals, | 0:50:44 | 0:50:47 | |
but look at the things that have made them what they are. | 0:50:47 | 0:50:50 | |
And when we do that, a pattern emerges. | 0:50:50 | 0:50:53 | |
For instance, all these animals eat a wide variety of foods | 0:50:57 | 0:51:02 | |
and need to master different techniques in order to obtain them. | 0:51:02 | 0:51:05 | |
They crack, they pluck and they hunt. | 0:51:07 | 0:51:12 | |
Our own omnivorous diet isn't too different. | 0:51:14 | 0:51:16 | |
And all these animals tend to be both predator and prey. | 0:51:19 | 0:51:23 | |
Which is also true of our own ancient ancestors | 0:51:26 | 0:51:30 | |
on the plains of Africa, millions of years ago. | 0:51:30 | 0:51:33 | |
So it seems that, to live flexibly, | 0:51:33 | 0:51:36 | |
you have to be able to think flexibly. | 0:51:36 | 0:51:39 | |
But there is something else that the supreme problem solvers - | 0:51:40 | 0:51:44 | |
the apes, the corvids and the parrots - share. | 0:51:44 | 0:51:47 | |
They live in groups. | 0:51:48 | 0:51:52 | |
So whether they walk, swim or fly, | 0:51:53 | 0:51:55 | |
the supreme problem solvers of the animal kingdom are not | 0:51:55 | 0:51:58 | |
so different from one another as we might have first imagined | 0:51:58 | 0:52:02 | |
and, although their minds are not as potent as ours, | 0:52:02 | 0:52:05 | |
their powers to reason | 0:52:05 | 0:52:07 | |
and even exercise imagination are quite remarkable. | 0:52:07 | 0:52:11 | |
But then, up until now, we have been looking at them as individuals. | 0:52:11 | 0:52:16 | |
What I want to understand next is do they have the capacity, | 0:52:16 | 0:52:20 | |
like humans, to actually share the solutions to problems? | 0:52:20 | 0:52:24 | |
In short, if they come up with a good idea, can they pass it on? | 0:52:24 | 0:52:28 | |
On New Caledonia, the crows have lots of good ideas. | 0:52:40 | 0:52:44 | |
They are precision tool makers. | 0:52:47 | 0:52:49 | |
This one is in the process of doing something | 0:52:51 | 0:52:54 | |
we would normally expect only of humans. | 0:52:54 | 0:52:58 | |
It's crafting a hook that it's going to use to catch its prey. | 0:52:58 | 0:53:02 | |
It's an astounding behaviour. | 0:53:03 | 0:53:06 | |
Dr Alex Taylor and his colleagues are now investigating whether | 0:53:11 | 0:53:14 | |
these birds are able to share their tool-making skills with each other. | 0:53:14 | 0:53:19 | |
That is amazing. | 0:53:21 | 0:53:23 | |
So they have sculpted a little hook out of another piece | 0:53:23 | 0:53:26 | |
of the twig that formerly would have run out here. | 0:53:26 | 0:53:28 | |
It's beautiful. It's almost like a primitive human tool, isn't it? | 0:53:28 | 0:53:33 | |
It does appear that way. | 0:53:33 | 0:53:35 | |
We talk about the imposition of three-dimensional form | 0:53:35 | 0:53:38 | |
onto a natural object. | 0:53:38 | 0:53:40 | |
That's something that humans have only been able to do | 0:53:40 | 0:53:43 | |
for the last 100,000 or 200,000 years | 0:53:43 | 0:53:45 | |
and here we have a crow doing something very similar. | 0:53:45 | 0:53:48 | |
And you can see how that can be useful because a crow would be able | 0:53:48 | 0:53:51 | |
to insert that into a hole and quite literally use that hook | 0:53:51 | 0:53:54 | |
to draw out a grub or anything else it was after. | 0:53:54 | 0:53:57 | |
Absolutely. | 0:53:57 | 0:53:59 | |
Made by a bird! That is absolutely brilliant! Absolutely brilliant! | 0:53:59 | 0:54:03 | |
And of even more interest to Alex are these - | 0:54:06 | 0:54:09 | |
intricately cut tools from the leaves of the pandanus tree. | 0:54:09 | 0:54:13 | |
Unlike the hooks, | 0:54:15 | 0:54:17 | |
there are distantly different types of pandanus tool. | 0:54:17 | 0:54:20 | |
They differ in complexity, | 0:54:20 | 0:54:23 | |
ranging from a simple leaf fragment to multistep implements like these. | 0:54:23 | 0:54:28 | |
And across the island, | 0:54:29 | 0:54:31 | |
different groups of crows use different types of these tools. | 0:54:31 | 0:54:35 | |
In the south, many different fragments are found | 0:54:38 | 0:54:42 | |
that, as we move north, | 0:54:42 | 0:54:44 | |
the crows start to favour more complex, multi-staged tools. | 0:54:44 | 0:54:49 | |
It means the different groups of crows | 0:54:50 | 0:54:53 | |
have their own ways of doing things. | 0:54:53 | 0:54:55 | |
In human society, we would call this culture. | 0:54:57 | 0:55:00 | |
What we are seeing across New Caledonia is populations | 0:55:03 | 0:55:06 | |
of crows that appear to have traditions | 0:55:06 | 0:55:08 | |
of making single-step tools or two-step or three-step tools | 0:55:08 | 0:55:11 | |
and these traditions are persisting over 10 or 15 years at least, | 0:55:11 | 0:55:14 | |
that's as much...how long we've been studying them for | 0:55:14 | 0:55:17 | |
and we believe they have been there for a lot longer. | 0:55:17 | 0:55:19 | |
It appears there's some kind of transmission of tool design | 0:55:19 | 0:55:22 | |
across the population. | 0:55:22 | 0:55:25 | |
A rare and fascinating glimpse of how this might happen | 0:55:26 | 0:55:29 | |
has been captured on camera. | 0:55:29 | 0:55:31 | |
Here an adult bird is using a stick to probe for grubs inside a log. | 0:55:31 | 0:55:36 | |
A youngster stands by, watching, | 0:55:40 | 0:55:43 | |
as the adult seems to demonstrate the right way to use the tool. | 0:55:43 | 0:55:47 | |
And when the adult departs, | 0:55:50 | 0:55:52 | |
she leaves behind the stick in the hole. | 0:55:52 | 0:55:55 | |
The youngster can now have a go itself. | 0:55:55 | 0:55:59 | |
Although this one has some way to go | 0:55:59 | 0:56:01 | |
before it becomes an expert like its parents. | 0:56:01 | 0:56:04 | |
It seems as though one way ideas can travel through the crow population | 0:56:05 | 0:56:10 | |
is via family groups, the social circle. | 0:56:10 | 0:56:13 | |
But Alex's research suggests something even more extraordinary - | 0:56:15 | 0:56:19 | |
that, with each new generation, the ideas don't stand still | 0:56:19 | 0:56:24 | |
but are honed and improved. | 0:56:24 | 0:56:26 | |
When we talk about this, we talk about the ratchet effect, | 0:56:30 | 0:56:33 | |
which is this idea that it's a really good idea | 0:56:33 | 0:56:36 | |
to be able to copy each other and, as a group, you can end up | 0:56:36 | 0:56:39 | |
being able to build better and better tools. | 0:56:39 | 0:56:42 | |
Obviously, we don't invent the wheel every generation ourselves, | 0:56:42 | 0:56:45 | |
we make it better and better. | 0:56:45 | 0:56:47 | |
Potentially, this is what's going on here with the crows. | 0:56:47 | 0:56:50 | |
There's no concrete evidence that any animal species | 0:56:50 | 0:56:52 | |
is able to actually show this ratcheting up of their technology, | 0:56:52 | 0:56:55 | |
to make it more and more sophisticated. | 0:56:55 | 0:56:57 | |
So it would just be crows and humans? | 0:56:57 | 0:56:59 | |
-No chimpanzees? Nothing? -At the moment, no. | 0:56:59 | 0:57:02 | |
It's something we think is unique to humans, | 0:57:02 | 0:57:04 | |
but maybe is going on with these crows as well. | 0:57:04 | 0:57:06 | |
The New Caledonian crow has only been studied since the early 1990s. | 0:57:10 | 0:57:16 | |
In that short period, scientists have revealed an animal mind | 0:57:17 | 0:57:21 | |
that rivals the problem-solving skills of our closest cousin, | 0:57:21 | 0:57:24 | |
the chimpanzee. | 0:57:24 | 0:57:26 | |
And for me, the really exciting thing | 0:57:28 | 0:57:32 | |
is that we are just beginning to understand these animals. | 0:57:32 | 0:57:36 | |
Who knows what else they might be capable of? | 0:57:36 | 0:57:39 | |
On this journey, I've come to an understanding | 0:57:41 | 0:57:44 | |
about what makes some animals cleverer than others | 0:57:44 | 0:57:47 | |
and even where that cleverness actually comes from. | 0:57:47 | 0:57:50 | |
And whilst we might reign supreme across the animal kingdom | 0:57:52 | 0:57:56 | |
when it comes to complex thought, we are certainly not alone. | 0:57:56 | 0:57:59 | |
It also seems we may not hold a monopoly | 0:57:59 | 0:58:02 | |
on the ability to share ideas and learn from one another. | 0:58:02 | 0:58:06 | |
So perhaps it's no surprise at all | 0:58:06 | 0:58:09 | |
that the most intelligent animals on our planet are the social ones. | 0:58:09 | 0:58:13 | |
And next time I'll be investigating | 0:58:16 | 0:58:18 | |
the minds of some very social animals. | 0:58:18 | 0:58:21 | |
I'll uncover the secrets of dolphin society, | 0:58:21 | 0:58:25 | |
discover how chimps deceive one another | 0:58:25 | 0:58:29 | |
and learn that we are not alone in mourning our dead. | 0:58:29 | 0:58:32 |