Browse content similar to Social Climbers. Check below for episodes and series from the same categories and more!
Line | From | To | |
---|---|---|---|
Monkeys, together with apes, are our closest relatives. | 0:00:45 | 0:00:50 | |
They have the richest social life of all mammals, | 0:00:50 | 0:00:53 | |
and much of their time is spent establishing relationships | 0:00:53 | 0:00:56 | |
and climbing the social ladder. | 0:00:56 | 0:01:00 | |
And that has had some remarkable consequences. | 0:01:00 | 0:01:04 | |
South America, | 0:01:26 | 0:01:28 | |
and a few capuchin monkeys are having a morning get-together. | 0:01:28 | 0:01:32 | |
Capuchins are very like the earliest of all monkeys, judging from fossils. | 0:01:32 | 0:01:38 | |
The basic pattern, you might say. | 0:01:38 | 0:01:40 | |
With grasping hands and gripping tails, they race through the forest | 0:01:43 | 0:01:47 | |
with astonishing speed and agility. | 0:01:47 | 0:01:50 | |
But it's their big brains that really give them an edge. | 0:01:52 | 0:01:56 | |
If there is one thing that is characteristic of monkeys, | 0:01:56 | 0:02:01 | |
it is their ability to spot an opportunity and then exploit it. | 0:02:01 | 0:02:05 | |
That's especially true of capuchins. | 0:02:05 | 0:02:09 | |
Of course, they'll grab a ripe fruit, | 0:02:09 | 0:02:12 | |
but they investigate everything in their path. Little escapes them. | 0:02:12 | 0:02:18 | |
But it's their inquisitiveness that really impresses you | 0:02:21 | 0:02:23 | |
when you follow them. | 0:02:23 | 0:02:25 | |
They look for food everywhere. | 0:02:27 | 0:02:30 | |
Their colour vision is excellent. | 0:02:34 | 0:02:37 | |
Their sense of smell, however, is no better than ours, | 0:02:37 | 0:02:40 | |
so to find food that is hidden from view, they use brains - not noses. | 0:02:40 | 0:02:45 | |
They can, clearly, imagine what might be lurking beneath dry leaves. | 0:02:52 | 0:02:57 | |
Out of sight is by no means out of mind. | 0:02:57 | 0:03:01 | |
A good memory must also help. | 0:03:01 | 0:03:03 | |
Recalling a previous experience | 0:03:03 | 0:03:05 | |
could make the difference between getting stung and getting honey. | 0:03:05 | 0:03:10 | |
The troop is just entering these mangroves. | 0:03:25 | 0:03:29 | |
Most mammals wouldn't find much to eat here, | 0:03:29 | 0:03:33 | |
but monkeys' big brains enable them to find things that others miss. | 0:03:33 | 0:03:38 | |
Only the bolder ones chance their hand with crabs. | 0:03:46 | 0:03:51 | |
It's quite brave to tackle sharp claws with delicate fingers. | 0:03:55 | 0:03:59 | |
A retreating tide exposes shellfish. | 0:04:02 | 0:04:06 | |
But eating those poses more problems. | 0:04:06 | 0:04:08 | |
The clams are full of meat, but it's all locked up behind closed doors. | 0:04:13 | 0:04:18 | |
However, grasping hands guided by a big brain can deal with that problem. | 0:04:22 | 0:04:27 | |
These clams are very tough. | 0:04:36 | 0:04:39 | |
I would have a lot of difficulty in opening them myself, | 0:04:39 | 0:04:42 | |
but these monkeys have learned that if you hit them hard enough, | 0:04:42 | 0:04:46 | |
the clam relaxes, the shells can be opened, | 0:04:46 | 0:04:49 | |
and they can get at the juicy meat inside. | 0:04:49 | 0:04:52 | |
The whole troop is in on the trick, | 0:04:59 | 0:05:03 | |
and now at least 20 of them are busy giving the clams the treatment. | 0:05:03 | 0:05:07 | |
Ten minutes on, and the shells of the clams are still tightly clenched. | 0:05:20 | 0:05:25 | |
This is hardly fast food. | 0:05:25 | 0:05:28 | |
The young watch attentively. | 0:05:31 | 0:05:34 | |
Most will eventually learn how to do it, | 0:05:34 | 0:05:38 | |
so the clam-cracking technique will pass to the next generation. | 0:05:38 | 0:05:41 | |
But it takes years for a young monkey to get it absolutely right. | 0:05:41 | 0:05:46 | |
Patience is important. Unfortunately, so is technique. | 0:05:49 | 0:05:55 | |
Capuchins, just like us, | 0:05:57 | 0:05:59 | |
have varied personalities and abilities, | 0:05:59 | 0:06:03 | |
and some never really get to grips with clam-cracking. | 0:06:03 | 0:06:06 | |
Those with a good technique AND perseverance | 0:06:19 | 0:06:22 | |
are now beginning to collect their rewards. | 0:06:22 | 0:06:25 | |
Although some aren't exactly thrilled by what they find. | 0:06:29 | 0:06:34 | |
It's not just food that monkeys collect from the forest. | 0:06:44 | 0:06:48 | |
The two bunches of leaves may look very much the same, | 0:06:48 | 0:06:52 | |
but this kind is used by the local people | 0:06:52 | 0:06:56 | |
as an antiseptic and insect repellent, | 0:06:56 | 0:06:59 | |
whereas this one has no medicinal qualities at all. | 0:06:59 | 0:07:04 | |
If I put them both down, I think I know which the monkeys will choose. | 0:07:04 | 0:07:09 | |
Those are pipa leaves - the ones with the insect-repelling sap. | 0:07:29 | 0:07:34 | |
Pipa is hard to find, so this big bunch will cause a lot of excitement. | 0:07:34 | 0:07:39 | |
Like many things in monkey life, | 0:07:48 | 0:07:51 | |
pipa rubbing is a major social event. | 0:07:51 | 0:07:54 | |
Those taking part disregard the differences | 0:07:54 | 0:07:57 | |
they might have had at other times and everyone joins in. | 0:07:57 | 0:08:01 | |
Do these capuchin really know that this behaviour | 0:08:38 | 0:08:42 | |
protects them against skin infections and mosquitoes? | 0:08:42 | 0:08:45 | |
Well, of course, it's impossible to say. | 0:08:45 | 0:08:48 | |
But they do it more frequently during the rainy season | 0:08:48 | 0:08:52 | |
when there are more skin infections and mosquitoes around | 0:08:52 | 0:08:56 | |
than at any other time during the year. | 0:08:56 | 0:08:58 | |
Not all capuchins use pipa. But once one discovers the plant's properties, | 0:08:59 | 0:09:05 | |
the knowledge quickly spreads, | 0:09:05 | 0:09:06 | |
so pipa-leaf rubbing becomes a group tradition. | 0:09:06 | 0:09:10 | |
Capuchins eat most things. | 0:09:14 | 0:09:17 | |
Some monkeys, however, have particular specialities. | 0:09:17 | 0:09:20 | |
These are uakaris, and they will eat fruit of an unusual kind. | 0:09:20 | 0:09:25 | |
The colour of their faces indicates their social position. | 0:09:27 | 0:09:31 | |
The brighter the scarlet, the more senior the animal. | 0:09:31 | 0:09:34 | |
The good colour vision that enables them to tell where they are socially, | 0:09:34 | 0:09:39 | |
also helps them to select fruit. | 0:09:39 | 0:09:41 | |
Unusually, they will eat fruits that are a vivid green and not yet ripe. | 0:09:43 | 0:09:49 | |
The monkeys in this forest can all eat slightly different foods, | 0:09:50 | 0:09:53 | |
allowing many species to live together side by side. | 0:09:53 | 0:09:57 | |
The saki has special teeth | 0:09:58 | 0:10:01 | |
which enable it to eat nuts that no other monkey can crack. | 0:10:01 | 0:10:05 | |
The spider monkey has long limbs and a grasping tail | 0:10:09 | 0:10:14 | |
which enable it to collect the very ripest fruit. | 0:10:14 | 0:10:17 | |
But there are foods in this forest | 0:10:19 | 0:10:21 | |
that not even a spider monkey can get to. | 0:10:21 | 0:10:24 | |
There is food for monkeys in every part of the rainforest, | 0:10:49 | 0:10:54 | |
but some of it is very difficult to reach. | 0:10:54 | 0:10:57 | |
I have only got up here into the canopy in a special crane, | 0:10:57 | 0:11:00 | |
and I am 120 feet above the ground. | 0:11:00 | 0:11:03 | |
And here, on the outermost twigs of the trees, | 0:11:03 | 0:11:06 | |
the stems are too thin to support a normal-sized monkey. | 0:11:06 | 0:11:11 | |
And yet, there is a lot of food up here. | 0:11:11 | 0:11:14 | |
Underneath these leaves - | 0:11:14 | 0:11:16 | |
a cocoon, a caterpillar, another leaf-eating insect. | 0:11:16 | 0:11:21 | |
But there is one kind of monkey that can get up here and eat these things. | 0:11:21 | 0:11:27 | |
It's a pygmy marmoset - the smallest monkey in the world, | 0:11:27 | 0:11:33 | |
no bigger than my hand. | 0:11:33 | 0:11:35 | |
Its feet are too small to grasp anything but the thinnest twigs. | 0:11:42 | 0:11:48 | |
Their needle-sharp claws prevent them from slipping. | 0:11:48 | 0:11:51 | |
As it stalks its insects it has to move with great stealth, | 0:12:00 | 0:12:05 | |
lest any vibration should scare them off. | 0:12:05 | 0:12:08 | |
But these hunters are so small | 0:12:08 | 0:12:11 | |
that not even the thinnest branches tremble under their weight. | 0:12:11 | 0:12:14 | |
All monkeys have forward-pointing eyes so that they can judge distance. | 0:12:29 | 0:12:34 | |
That's invaluable when moving around in the trees. | 0:12:34 | 0:12:37 | |
But this tiny little hunter uses that ability also to pounce. | 0:12:37 | 0:12:42 | |
But their staple diet is altogether more extraordinary. | 0:12:50 | 0:12:55 | |
You might think that the lumps on this tree are a natural feature, | 0:12:55 | 0:13:00 | |
but, in fact, they are wounds inflicted by pygmy marmosets | 0:13:00 | 0:13:04 | |
that come to this tree trunk every day. | 0:13:04 | 0:13:07 | |
It's eating gum, | 0:13:16 | 0:13:18 | |
which it has forced the tree to produce by gnawing its bark. | 0:13:18 | 0:13:23 | |
And what's more, it is making sure that there will be gum tomorrow, | 0:13:23 | 0:13:29 | |
because it is also re-opening the wounds that the tree has healed up. | 0:13:29 | 0:13:35 | |
The marmosets keep opening these wounds | 0:13:35 | 0:13:39 | |
and the tree keeps closing them | 0:13:39 | 0:13:41 | |
until so much scar tissue builds up that circular mounds appear. | 0:13:41 | 0:13:46 | |
Generations of pygmy marmosets have done that on this particular tree, | 0:13:46 | 0:13:52 | |
resulting in these huge columns of lumps running right up the trunk. | 0:13:52 | 0:13:58 | |
A well-managed gum tree is an asset for a group of marmosets, | 0:13:59 | 0:14:03 | |
making it a tempting target for thieves. | 0:14:03 | 0:14:06 | |
And a rival group is preparing to invade. | 0:14:09 | 0:14:12 | |
The intruders charge and the owners flee, | 0:14:16 | 0:14:20 | |
leaving their precious gum undefended. | 0:14:20 | 0:14:23 | |
While the invaders steal the gum, | 0:14:27 | 0:14:30 | |
the displaced residents start scent-marking in the trees nearby. | 0:14:30 | 0:14:35 | |
This seems to reinforce the bonds between them | 0:14:35 | 0:14:37 | |
and stiffen their resolve to mount a counter-attack. | 0:14:37 | 0:14:41 | |
It works. The home team are back on their tree. | 0:14:46 | 0:14:49 | |
But you can share a food resource that is cropped by others | 0:14:59 | 0:15:03 | |
without having a face-to-face confrontation. | 0:15:03 | 0:15:06 | |
You might think that when the sun goes down, | 0:15:09 | 0:15:11 | |
all monkeys would be settling down to sleep. | 0:15:11 | 0:15:14 | |
That's almost true, but not quite. | 0:15:14 | 0:15:17 | |
There's only one monkey in the world that collects its food at night. | 0:15:21 | 0:15:25 | |
And precisely because it IS nocturnal, | 0:15:25 | 0:15:28 | |
it is very seldom seen and still little known. | 0:15:28 | 0:15:31 | |
But there is a family of them living in this tree behind me. | 0:15:31 | 0:15:35 | |
They are douroucoulis. | 0:15:35 | 0:15:36 | |
They are also known, for obvious reasons, as owl monkeys. | 0:15:40 | 0:15:44 | |
Their eyes may be huge, | 0:15:48 | 0:15:50 | |
but, in fact, their night vision is not particularly good, | 0:15:50 | 0:15:53 | |
suggesting that their ancestors lived by day | 0:15:53 | 0:15:56 | |
and that douroucoulis became nocturnal relatively recently. | 0:15:56 | 0:16:01 | |
During the day, the nectar-rich flowers on which they feed | 0:16:06 | 0:16:10 | |
are guarded aggressively by other monkeys. | 0:16:10 | 0:16:13 | |
By coming out only at night, | 0:16:13 | 0:16:15 | |
douroucoulis avoid having to fight for a share and can feed in peace. | 0:16:15 | 0:16:18 | |
Day breaks. | 0:16:41 | 0:16:42 | |
The douroucoulis retire | 0:16:44 | 0:16:46 | |
and a new set of fruit- and nectar-feeding monkeys awake. | 0:16:46 | 0:16:49 | |
Tamarins set off to collect their share. | 0:16:52 | 0:16:55 | |
But this forest is not rich in such food, | 0:16:55 | 0:16:58 | |
and though they live in small groups, finding all they need takes time. | 0:16:58 | 0:17:03 | |
The female always produces twins, which travel with the family. | 0:17:04 | 0:17:09 | |
But she has two male partners and she uses her tongue to signal | 0:17:12 | 0:17:17 | |
that it's time that one of them took over the twins. | 0:17:17 | 0:17:20 | |
Both are eager to do so, for both mated with her | 0:17:30 | 0:17:34 | |
and so each has cause to regard the babies as his. | 0:17:34 | 0:17:37 | |
But the twins have other ideas. They want to stay with Mum. | 0:17:45 | 0:17:49 | |
She gets rid of them at last, and it is important that she does so. | 0:18:00 | 0:18:04 | |
She has to provide them with milk, so she must eat much more than usual | 0:18:04 | 0:18:09 | |
and she would find that difficult | 0:18:09 | 0:18:11 | |
if she were burdened by her babies all the time. | 0:18:11 | 0:18:13 | |
The situation is made harder, | 0:18:20 | 0:18:23 | |
because there are other kinds of tamarins here as well. | 0:18:23 | 0:18:26 | |
This is one of them - a saddleback tamarin. | 0:18:26 | 0:18:30 | |
Monkeys usually guard their food ferociously, | 0:18:30 | 0:18:33 | |
but these two kinds of tamarin often feed side by side. | 0:18:33 | 0:18:38 | |
And when one leaves, the other follows. | 0:18:39 | 0:18:42 | |
The emperors go first, and the saddlebacks - | 0:18:49 | 0:18:52 | |
with their shorter, stumpier legs - try hard to keep up. | 0:18:52 | 0:18:56 | |
The travelling party shares a quick refuel. | 0:19:01 | 0:19:04 | |
Both kinds of tamarin, being small and energetic, need frequent snacks. | 0:19:04 | 0:19:09 | |
They share another thing, too. They have a common enemy. | 0:19:10 | 0:19:14 | |
They give alarm calls that both species understand. | 0:19:14 | 0:19:18 | |
TAMARIN SHRIEKS | 0:19:18 | 0:19:20 | |
A tayra - a kind of giant weasel. | 0:19:20 | 0:19:23 | |
Not the most dangerous predator in this forest, | 0:19:23 | 0:19:27 | |
but deadly to a bite-sized monkey. | 0:19:27 | 0:19:29 | |
It's certainly safer to travel in groups, but having to share food | 0:19:40 | 0:19:44 | |
is a high price to pay for a warning system. | 0:19:44 | 0:19:48 | |
There is, however, another rather more subtle advantage. | 0:19:48 | 0:19:53 | |
Each flower needs time to refill its reservoir of nectar | 0:19:56 | 0:20:01 | |
after a visitor has drunk from it. | 0:20:01 | 0:20:04 | |
If either kind of tamarin foraged separately, | 0:20:04 | 0:20:08 | |
they might make a hazardous journey, | 0:20:08 | 0:20:10 | |
only to reach a tree that had recently been emptied by the other. | 0:20:10 | 0:20:14 | |
That would be disastrous for an animal that has to feed so regularly. | 0:20:14 | 0:20:19 | |
It's far better for tamarins to travel together | 0:20:20 | 0:20:23 | |
and then share the food when they arrive. | 0:20:23 | 0:20:26 | |
Howler monkeys - ten times the size of a tamarin. | 0:20:37 | 0:20:41 | |
They also eat flowers, given the chance, | 0:20:43 | 0:20:47 | |
but these are not always available. | 0:20:47 | 0:20:49 | |
In tropical forest, at least leaves can be found all year round. | 0:20:49 | 0:20:55 | |
Howler monkeys specialise on these. | 0:20:55 | 0:20:58 | |
The best leaves are up in the canopy, | 0:21:00 | 0:21:03 | |
so that's the best place to go if you want to watch howlers close-up. | 0:21:03 | 0:21:07 | |
Leaves are not all the same. | 0:21:16 | 0:21:18 | |
Some are poisonous, even to a howler monkey. | 0:21:18 | 0:21:22 | |
Of those that are edible, some are nicer than others. | 0:21:22 | 0:21:26 | |
But, once again, that monkey ability to see in colour is a great help. | 0:21:26 | 0:21:31 | |
These red ones are young and succulent, | 0:21:36 | 0:21:40 | |
and would be very good to eat, if they weren't protected by poisons. | 0:21:40 | 0:21:45 | |
These mature ones are green and the poison in them has faded, | 0:21:45 | 0:21:50 | |
but they're fibrous and woody and NOT very good to eat. | 0:21:50 | 0:21:54 | |
A leaf-eater wants something in between, like this. | 0:21:54 | 0:21:59 | |
Imagine how difficult it would be to pick the right one | 0:22:01 | 0:22:06 | |
if you didn't have colour vision. | 0:22:06 | 0:22:09 | |
Even the best leaves are not easy to digest, | 0:22:17 | 0:22:21 | |
so, although howlers spend a short time collecting food, | 0:22:21 | 0:22:24 | |
they spend a long time digesting it. | 0:22:24 | 0:22:27 | |
In fact, they spend half of every day just lying around, | 0:22:27 | 0:22:32 | |
like cows, chewing the cud. | 0:22:32 | 0:22:34 | |
Even after all that digestion, leaves are not very nutritious, | 0:22:50 | 0:22:55 | |
so howlers can't waste energy chasing rivals through the treetops. | 0:22:55 | 0:23:00 | |
They use a rather more labour-saving way of doing that. | 0:23:00 | 0:23:04 | |
GRUNTING | 0:23:09 | 0:23:12 | |
Howlers have a specially enlarged bone in the throat, the hyoid, | 0:23:22 | 0:23:27 | |
that enables them to make this extraordinary song. | 0:23:27 | 0:23:31 | |
It's one of the loudest noises made by any animal | 0:23:31 | 0:23:35 | |
and the whole family joins in almost every evening. | 0:23:35 | 0:23:40 | |
They work themselves into a frenzy, | 0:23:40 | 0:23:43 | |
but since neither they, nor their rivals actually move anywhere, | 0:23:43 | 0:23:47 | |
it's very hard to say who's winning. | 0:23:47 | 0:23:51 | |
It may be that this howling serves to strengthen family solidarity | 0:23:59 | 0:24:03 | |
and deter intruders. | 0:24:03 | 0:24:05 | |
RASPING GRUNTS | 0:24:22 | 0:24:25 | |
That may sound a bit like a howler monkey, | 0:24:32 | 0:24:35 | |
but I've left South America and I'm in Africa. | 0:24:35 | 0:24:38 | |
The howler's African equivalent is the black and white colobus. | 0:24:38 | 0:24:43 | |
Many different kinds of monkeys are able to share the same forest, | 0:24:45 | 0:24:50 | |
because they find their food in different ways. | 0:24:50 | 0:24:53 | |
Red colobus' stomachs allows them to eat unripe fruit and leaves, | 0:24:54 | 0:25:00 | |
whereas sooty mangabeys, like saki monkeys, | 0:25:00 | 0:25:03 | |
have powerful jaws that can break open really tough foods. | 0:25:03 | 0:25:07 | |
The rest of the monkeys have a more general diet, like capuchins, | 0:25:11 | 0:25:15 | |
and belong to a group called the guenons. | 0:25:15 | 0:25:18 | |
All guenons eat fruit and insects, | 0:25:18 | 0:25:22 | |
but each looks for them at its own level in the forest. | 0:25:22 | 0:25:26 | |
There are 17 different kinds of guenon in Africa. | 0:25:26 | 0:25:30 | |
They all have similar-shaped bodies, | 0:25:30 | 0:25:33 | |
but each has its own colours and patterns | 0:25:33 | 0:25:36 | |
that serve as signals allowing them to recognise friends and rivals. | 0:25:36 | 0:25:42 | |
They are the most colourful group of monkeys in the world. | 0:25:42 | 0:25:45 | |
But colour signals are not a good way of sending urgent messages | 0:25:51 | 0:25:55 | |
in places where the visibility is poor, | 0:25:55 | 0:25:57 | |
and that's certainly the case here in the Tai Forest in West Africa. | 0:25:57 | 0:26:02 | |
Failing to spot another monkey is one thing, but if you miss a warning, | 0:26:03 | 0:26:07 | |
the consequences could be fatal. | 0:26:07 | 0:26:09 | |
So how do monkeys keep out of harm's way? | 0:26:19 | 0:26:23 | |
I'm travelling with one of the most extraordinary | 0:26:25 | 0:26:29 | |
anti-predator alliances in the world. | 0:26:29 | 0:26:31 | |
Around me is a troop of sooty mangabeys and mongooses. | 0:26:32 | 0:26:38 | |
They're keeping an eye out for danger on the ground. | 0:26:38 | 0:26:42 | |
In the branches above me, there are at least five species of monkey, | 0:26:46 | 0:26:52 | |
all ready to detect a threat from the sky or from within the canopy. | 0:26:52 | 0:26:57 | |
In this alliance, monkeys of many kinds travel side by side. | 0:26:58 | 0:27:03 | |
It's a huge safety network | 0:27:04 | 0:27:06 | |
that extends from the canopy right down to the forest floor. | 0:27:06 | 0:27:10 | |
With so many pairs of eyes at every level, | 0:27:12 | 0:27:16 | |
someone is sure to spot a threat, no matter where it comes from. | 0:27:16 | 0:27:20 | |
There's little squabbling over food between different species | 0:27:26 | 0:27:30 | |
because each finds it in a different way. | 0:27:30 | 0:27:32 | |
Visibility is so poor here that they communicate almost entirely by sound. | 0:27:37 | 0:27:42 | |
Unlike visual signs, a sound reaches every monkey in a split second. | 0:27:42 | 0:27:47 | |
It's the best means of staying in contact or warning of danger. | 0:27:47 | 0:27:52 | |
Diana monkeys forage near the top of the canopy, | 0:27:53 | 0:27:57 | |
so they're usually first to spot danger from the air. | 0:27:57 | 0:28:00 | |
Crowned eagles are never far away here, but if one comes too close, | 0:28:04 | 0:28:09 | |
a specific alarm will be sounded, telling monkeys to drop for cover. | 0:28:09 | 0:28:14 | |
Danger above! Monkeys plummet down without even looking up. | 0:28:26 | 0:28:31 | |
The alliance has saved another member. | 0:28:34 | 0:28:36 | |
All these monkeys have different alarm calls for different predators. | 0:28:38 | 0:28:43 | |
Watch this! | 0:28:43 | 0:28:45 | |
SQUEALING | 0:29:11 | 0:29:13 | |
GRUNTING | 0:29:17 | 0:29:19 | |
That's the leopard alarm call of the Diana monkey. | 0:29:25 | 0:29:29 | |
GRUNTING That different one is the spot-nose. | 0:29:29 | 0:29:34 | |
But the important point is that all species of monkey in the alliance | 0:29:34 | 0:29:39 | |
recognise one another's call and know the nature of the enemy. | 0:29:39 | 0:29:43 | |
That's important | 0:29:43 | 0:29:45 | |
because each predator requires its own particular response. | 0:29:45 | 0:29:50 | |
All these monkeys know there's a leopard around | 0:29:52 | 0:29:56 | |
and you might think, therefore, that they would flee, | 0:29:56 | 0:29:59 | |
but they don't - they're advancing towards it, | 0:29:59 | 0:30:02 | |
and that's because the leopard is an ambush predator. | 0:30:02 | 0:30:06 | |
Once it's spotted, it usually gives up and goes away. | 0:30:06 | 0:30:10 | |
Monkeys are drawn by the call from a large area. | 0:30:12 | 0:30:15 | |
They even drop lower, so they can keep the cat in full view. | 0:30:15 | 0:30:19 | |
And here they will stay, on maximum alert, | 0:30:23 | 0:30:26 | |
until the leopard finally gives up and leaves them in peace. | 0:30:26 | 0:30:30 | |
The ancient city of Polonnaruwa in Sri Lanka. | 0:30:44 | 0:30:48 | |
The civilisation that built it 1,000 years ago has long since crumbled, | 0:30:48 | 0:30:53 | |
but one society that lived here then is still here now and thriving. | 0:30:53 | 0:30:58 | |
Toque macaques have walked these walls for centuries, | 0:31:02 | 0:31:07 | |
but only in the last few decades | 0:31:07 | 0:31:09 | |
have we started to understand their complex society. | 0:31:09 | 0:31:12 | |
This is one of the most studied monkey groups of all time. | 0:31:23 | 0:31:27 | |
For 30 years, their daily existence has been recorded in detail. | 0:31:27 | 0:31:32 | |
That has given us great insight into their private lives. | 0:31:32 | 0:31:37 | |
These youngsters are playing as equals, | 0:31:39 | 0:31:42 | |
but they have very different futures ahead of them. | 0:31:42 | 0:31:45 | |
There's a ruthless class system in monkey society. | 0:31:45 | 0:31:48 | |
Those born in high-ranking families have a massive head-start in life. | 0:31:48 | 0:31:53 | |
Emelda is one of them. She is highborn. | 0:31:56 | 0:32:00 | |
Others have been feasting on berries, but she's come too late to get any. | 0:32:00 | 0:32:05 | |
The berries are stashed in the cheek-pouches of others. | 0:32:05 | 0:32:09 | |
Emelda spots Poppin. She is far larger and older than Emelda, | 0:32:09 | 0:32:15 | |
but she is from a lower-ranking family, | 0:32:15 | 0:32:18 | |
entitling Emelda to take anything of Poppin's that she wants, | 0:32:18 | 0:32:22 | |
including the food right out of her mouth. | 0:32:22 | 0:32:26 | |
Poppin makes it as hard for Emelda as she dare, | 0:32:33 | 0:32:36 | |
but if she resists too strongly, she risks a beating from the rest. | 0:32:36 | 0:32:42 | |
In monkey society, it helps to be highborn. | 0:32:54 | 0:32:58 | |
It also helps to be intelligent, | 0:32:58 | 0:33:00 | |
and that is particularly apparent during the mating season. | 0:33:00 | 0:33:04 | |
Toque macaques try to attract one another's attention | 0:33:06 | 0:33:09 | |
by fluttering their mauve eyelids and flashing their teeth. | 0:33:09 | 0:33:14 | |
But although there are willing males everywhere you look, | 0:33:18 | 0:33:23 | |
there is a shortage of females ready to mate. | 0:33:23 | 0:33:27 | |
Females are only fertile for a few weeks in the year. | 0:33:31 | 0:33:35 | |
During this short period, lots of males want to mate with them, | 0:33:35 | 0:33:39 | |
but hierarchy does not allow that. | 0:33:39 | 0:33:42 | |
This male in front of me, | 0:33:42 | 0:33:44 | |
after years of political manoeuvring and a good deal of muscle, | 0:33:44 | 0:33:48 | |
has become alpha male, and he has first pick, | 0:33:48 | 0:33:53 | |
but there are always some young males who try to beat the system. | 0:33:53 | 0:33:59 | |
Booster has a lesser rank | 0:33:59 | 0:34:01 | |
and that prevents him from openly mating with a fertile female. | 0:34:01 | 0:34:05 | |
His best chance is a secret liaison, | 0:34:05 | 0:34:08 | |
but the alpha male has recruited many allies, | 0:34:08 | 0:34:11 | |
and these males will stop any female who tries to sneak away | 0:34:11 | 0:34:16 | |
and reprimand her, if necessary. | 0:34:16 | 0:34:19 | |
This security team allows the alpha to concentrate | 0:34:24 | 0:34:28 | |
on the privileges of his position. | 0:34:28 | 0:34:31 | |
But Booster's chance has come. | 0:34:36 | 0:34:39 | |
The alpha male's allies don't have eyes in the backs of their heads, | 0:34:39 | 0:34:44 | |
and females, like Shanti here, see Booster as a future alpha. | 0:34:44 | 0:34:49 | |
Allowing him to father their young could be a shrewd investment. | 0:34:51 | 0:34:54 | |
First, she needs to attract Booster's attention. | 0:34:56 | 0:35:00 | |
Shanti is flirting. | 0:35:02 | 0:35:05 | |
Her glances are subtle, because she doesn't want to get caught, | 0:35:05 | 0:35:10 | |
but they're unmistakable, if you know what you're looking for, | 0:35:10 | 0:35:13 | |
and Booster certainly does. | 0:35:13 | 0:35:16 | |
He needs to signal his intentions to Shanti, | 0:35:18 | 0:35:21 | |
but feign disinterest should the alpha male look in his direction. | 0:35:21 | 0:35:25 | |
But the alpha has other females to attend to | 0:35:30 | 0:35:34 | |
and his allies are nowhere to be seen. | 0:35:34 | 0:35:38 | |
Shanti gives Booster the eye. | 0:35:38 | 0:35:40 | |
They must be discreet and quick. Shanti's absence may be noticed. | 0:35:50 | 0:35:55 | |
If Booster DOES take over, he may look favourably on Shanti's young. | 0:36:05 | 0:36:11 | |
They might, after all, be his. | 0:36:11 | 0:36:14 | |
A big brain certainly helps you to take advantage of others, | 0:36:21 | 0:36:26 | |
and the larger the group you live in, the bigger your brain needs to be. | 0:36:26 | 0:36:31 | |
But it was a dramatic change to the Earth itself | 0:36:34 | 0:36:39 | |
that allowed monkeys to climb up to the next rung of the social ladder. | 0:36:39 | 0:36:44 | |
10 million years ago, the climate of the world changed. | 0:36:47 | 0:36:52 | |
Many places became much drier. | 0:36:52 | 0:36:54 | |
The rainforests shrank | 0:36:54 | 0:36:57 | |
and were replaced by scrub and open grassland. | 0:36:57 | 0:37:01 | |
But there was a lot to eat in this new terrain. | 0:37:01 | 0:37:05 | |
Some of the monkeys came down from the branches | 0:37:05 | 0:37:09 | |
and went out to find it in the open. | 0:37:09 | 0:37:12 | |
The first monkeys to walk out here would have found some familiar foods, | 0:37:14 | 0:37:18 | |
flowers and seasonal berries, | 0:37:18 | 0:37:21 | |
but today baboons collect all kinds of other things - | 0:37:21 | 0:37:25 | |
bulbs, cactus, | 0:37:25 | 0:37:27 | |
even rabbits. | 0:37:27 | 0:37:29 | |
The grasslands are a great place for an opportunist. | 0:37:32 | 0:37:35 | |
Baboons use their fingers and brains | 0:37:35 | 0:37:38 | |
to collect an extraordinary variety of foods. | 0:37:38 | 0:37:42 | |
Insect grubs can be picked up | 0:37:42 | 0:37:45 | |
from the edge of a soda lake in Kenya's Rift Valley. | 0:37:45 | 0:37:49 | |
Flamingos gather in vast numbers and generate a rich layer of compost | 0:37:49 | 0:37:54 | |
that contains plenty of food for those patient enough | 0:37:54 | 0:37:57 | |
to pick through it. | 0:37:57 | 0:37:59 | |
But a few baboons have become more ambitious. | 0:37:59 | 0:38:02 | |
Only this troop of baboons catch flamingos. | 0:39:23 | 0:39:26 | |
It's just five years since the first of them discovered this new food | 0:39:26 | 0:39:30 | |
and worked out how to get it, | 0:39:30 | 0:39:33 | |
but the skill has spread quickly | 0:39:33 | 0:39:35 | |
and now the whole troop are dedicated flamingo hunters. | 0:39:35 | 0:39:38 | |
Life in the open has its disadvantages, too. | 0:39:44 | 0:39:48 | |
There are more big predators around, | 0:39:48 | 0:39:50 | |
so baboons protect themselves by assembling in large groups. | 0:39:50 | 0:39:54 | |
As among macaques, living in big groups causes social problems. | 0:39:56 | 0:40:01 | |
And here there's an even more complex social structure, | 0:40:01 | 0:40:04 | |
within which males compete for females. | 0:40:04 | 0:40:06 | |
When a female becomes sexually available, | 0:40:09 | 0:40:12 | |
her bottom swells and turns bright pink. | 0:40:12 | 0:40:15 | |
All the males can read the message, but she only offers herself | 0:40:15 | 0:40:18 | |
to males that have looked after her during the rest of the year. | 0:40:18 | 0:40:22 | |
This male is attempting to join the troop, | 0:40:25 | 0:40:29 | |
but it may be some time before he can father any offspring. | 0:40:29 | 0:40:33 | |
First, he has to learn the social order in this complex community | 0:40:33 | 0:40:37 | |
and a good way to start is to befriend one of the senior females. | 0:40:37 | 0:40:41 | |
The ledge can get very crowded, | 0:40:43 | 0:40:46 | |
and the presence of the alpha male is intimidating. | 0:40:46 | 0:40:50 | |
Even deciding where to sit is a political decision. | 0:40:50 | 0:40:55 | |
The new male inevitably invades the personal space of others. | 0:40:55 | 0:40:59 | |
Tempers flare. | 0:40:59 | 0:41:01 | |
SHRIEKING AND CHATTERING | 0:41:01 | 0:41:04 | |
Life for a new arrival can be very stressful. | 0:41:16 | 0:41:19 | |
Established males rely on a network of allies for support. | 0:41:22 | 0:41:27 | |
The most successful of them invested heavily in such social relationships. | 0:41:27 | 0:41:31 | |
But knowing who to build them with takes intelligence. | 0:41:31 | 0:41:35 | |
Grooming is a good way to maintain friendships. | 0:41:36 | 0:41:40 | |
Looking after babies will also score you points. | 0:41:40 | 0:41:43 | |
Child-caring brings another benefit. | 0:41:45 | 0:41:48 | |
Babies are protected in baboon society, | 0:41:48 | 0:41:52 | |
so no-one will hit you if you're carrying one. | 0:41:52 | 0:41:54 | |
That allows smart baboons to use infants like shields | 0:41:54 | 0:41:59 | |
when they feel threatened. | 0:41:59 | 0:42:01 | |
It takes time and brain power | 0:42:01 | 0:42:05 | |
to rise to the top in a large group. | 0:42:05 | 0:42:08 | |
It's easy to imagine how a big brain could help you in baboon society. | 0:42:26 | 0:42:31 | |
But if social complexity IS the driving force | 0:42:31 | 0:42:35 | |
behind the growth of the primate brain, | 0:42:35 | 0:42:38 | |
then it SHOULD be that brain size in primates | 0:42:38 | 0:42:41 | |
corresponds closely to group size. | 0:42:41 | 0:42:44 | |
This is the skull of a baboon | 0:42:48 | 0:42:51 | |
and this lump of Plasticine | 0:42:51 | 0:42:54 | |
represents the brain which the skull once contained. | 0:42:54 | 0:42:58 | |
This represents the brain of a bush baby. | 0:42:58 | 0:43:02 | |
It's essentially a solitary animal, | 0:43:02 | 0:43:05 | |
but even allowing for its small body size, its brain is tiny. | 0:43:05 | 0:43:10 | |
This, slightly larger, is the brain of a colobus - group size 15. | 0:43:10 | 0:43:15 | |
This, bigger still, a guenon - group size 25. | 0:43:15 | 0:43:21 | |
And back to our baboon - group size 50. | 0:43:21 | 0:43:26 | |
The relationship is so close, | 0:43:26 | 0:43:29 | |
that were you to give a skull to a researcher who works with monkeys | 0:43:29 | 0:43:33 | |
even though they didn't know what kind of monkey it belonged to, | 0:43:33 | 0:43:37 | |
they could accurately predict the size of group in which it lived. | 0:43:37 | 0:43:41 | |
500 miles away in the highlands of Ethiopia | 0:43:46 | 0:43:50 | |
live the largest monkey groups of all. | 0:43:50 | 0:43:54 | |
Geladas - the world's only grazing monkeys. | 0:44:02 | 0:44:07 | |
There's a lot of grass up here | 0:44:07 | 0:44:10 | |
so geladas flourish and gather into enormous herds. | 0:44:10 | 0:44:14 | |
They don't have nipping teeth like sheep or rabbits | 0:44:14 | 0:44:18 | |
so they have to collect their grass by hand. | 0:44:18 | 0:44:22 | |
This is done best by sitting down | 0:44:22 | 0:44:25 | |
and they spend much of their lives shuffling along on their backsides. | 0:44:25 | 0:44:30 | |
Because they sit down so much, | 0:44:35 | 0:44:38 | |
their sexual displays have moved to their chests, | 0:44:38 | 0:44:41 | |
where other geladas can see them. | 0:44:41 | 0:44:44 | |
These indicate strength and virility | 0:44:44 | 0:44:48 | |
and allow individuals to assess each other's status at a glance, | 0:44:48 | 0:44:52 | |
so avoiding unnecessary confrontations. | 0:44:52 | 0:44:56 | |
Within these herds are smaller groups, | 0:44:57 | 0:44:59 | |
like those formed by other monkeys. | 0:44:59 | 0:45:02 | |
Related females are the core and a male harem leader watches over them. | 0:45:02 | 0:45:08 | |
And he has to be very vigilant. | 0:45:08 | 0:45:11 | |
A pack of bachelor males with one goal in mind - | 0:45:16 | 0:45:21 | |
to displace the holder of a harem. | 0:45:21 | 0:45:24 | |
The bachelors avoid males with the brightest chests - | 0:45:28 | 0:45:31 | |
they are in their prime and will have the full support of their family. | 0:45:31 | 0:45:34 | |
But less impressive males will have to prove their worth in battle. | 0:45:41 | 0:45:46 | |
A lip-flip from one of the bachelors is a threat. Flip answers flip. | 0:45:49 | 0:45:53 | |
There's going to be trouble. | 0:45:53 | 0:45:56 | |
These fights are largely for show and rarely result in injury, | 0:46:33 | 0:46:38 | |
but they do waste valuable grazing time. | 0:46:38 | 0:46:42 | |
Eating grass is time-consuming and also occupies your hands, | 0:46:42 | 0:46:46 | |
so it's hard to find time for grooming. | 0:46:46 | 0:46:49 | |
So geladas maintain their friendships in another way - | 0:46:49 | 0:46:54 | |
they chatter to one another. | 0:46:54 | 0:46:57 | |
CHIRPS AND MURMURS | 0:46:57 | 0:47:00 | |
These noises are, in effect, VOCAL grooming | 0:47:02 | 0:47:06 | |
and enable geladas to communicate with many individuals at once. | 0:47:06 | 0:47:12 | |
And this is the secret of their ability to live in huge groups. | 0:47:12 | 0:47:16 | |
Their continuous chatter SOUNDS like a language, | 0:47:19 | 0:47:23 | |
but we have no idea what sort of information they are exchanging. | 0:47:23 | 0:47:26 | |
One thing, however, IS likely - | 0:47:28 | 0:47:31 | |
it was the need to communicate detailed social information | 0:47:31 | 0:47:36 | |
between many individuals | 0:47:36 | 0:47:39 | |
that led to the evolution of language in our own species. | 0:47:39 | 0:47:42 | |
So although monkeys living in the tree tops have rich and varied lives, | 0:47:45 | 0:47:50 | |
it's the ones that came down to the ground and formed large groups | 0:47:50 | 0:47:55 | |
that have the most complex and communicative societies of all - | 0:47:55 | 0:48:00 | |
a fact not without significance for our own ancestry. | 0:48:00 | 0:48:04 | |
Those monkeys cracking shellfish and anointing themselves | 0:48:23 | 0:48:26 | |
with a natural insecticide | 0:48:26 | 0:48:29 | |
were showing a kind of behaviour that, not so long ago, | 0:48:29 | 0:48:32 | |
was thought to be exclusively human. | 0:48:32 | 0:48:35 | |
But over the past 40 years, | 0:48:35 | 0:48:37 | |
intensive studies have shown that monkeys have a surprising complexity | 0:48:37 | 0:48:42 | |
in their tool use, in their language, and in their societies. | 0:48:42 | 0:48:46 | |
Primatologist Jane Goodall | 0:48:47 | 0:48:50 | |
is famous for her ground-breaking studies of wild chimpanzees, | 0:48:50 | 0:48:54 | |
but in 1960, she also began one of the first field studies of monkeys - | 0:48:54 | 0:48:59 | |
the olive baboons of Tanzania's Gombe Stream National Park. | 0:48:59 | 0:49:04 | |
Many monkey species have large, heavily armed males. | 0:49:06 | 0:49:09 | |
They seem to be constantly fighting | 0:49:09 | 0:49:12 | |
and soon the researchers identified the rank of different males | 0:49:12 | 0:49:15 | |
in a turbulent hierarchy of threats and challenges. | 0:49:15 | 0:49:19 | |
It is the young, prime male, entering a new troop | 0:49:23 | 0:49:26 | |
and eventually confronting the existing leader | 0:49:26 | 0:49:29 | |
who becomes the new, undisputed ruler of a troop. | 0:49:29 | 0:49:34 | |
Here, a male's rank depends on his dominance, | 0:49:36 | 0:49:39 | |
with the alpha male at the top. | 0:49:39 | 0:49:42 | |
Across Africa, biologists revealed baboon troops as complex, | 0:49:42 | 0:49:46 | |
highly-ordered and stable societies, containing long-term relationships. | 0:49:46 | 0:49:51 | |
This includes a key role for the females in the species | 0:49:55 | 0:49:59 | |
studied by Chadden Hunter - | 0:49:59 | 0:50:01 | |
the gelada baboon. | 0:50:01 | 0:50:03 | |
We have about 600 here filling up this valley, | 0:50:03 | 0:50:07 | |
so just this ocean of fur. | 0:50:07 | 0:50:09 | |
It's really hard to see just how many monkeys are here in one place. | 0:50:09 | 0:50:15 | |
Family groups are basically sisters who stick together - | 0:50:15 | 0:50:19 | |
say, four or five sisters. | 0:50:19 | 0:50:21 | |
They'll pick a male, he comes into the family, | 0:50:21 | 0:50:25 | |
has to toe the line with them - | 0:50:25 | 0:50:27 | |
basically protect them, protect the kids whenever they want him to. | 0:50:27 | 0:50:32 | |
On the side of the group | 0:50:32 | 0:50:35 | |
is this tight-knit kind of street gang of mean-looking bachelors | 0:50:35 | 0:50:40 | |
and if the sisters aren't happy with their male | 0:50:40 | 0:50:44 | |
then they'll chuck him out and get a new guy. | 0:50:44 | 0:50:48 | |
Even when there's relative stability, | 0:50:48 | 0:50:52 | |
males are constantly under pressure. | 0:50:52 | 0:50:54 | |
The alpha male can never relax. | 0:50:54 | 0:50:57 | |
By measuring hormone levels, | 0:50:58 | 0:51:02 | |
Professor Bob Sapolsky has shown that, just like humans, | 0:51:02 | 0:51:05 | |
baboons suffer from stress and stress-related diseases. | 0:51:05 | 0:51:10 | |
Baboons and us are very similar. | 0:51:10 | 0:51:12 | |
WE don't get ulcers cos we have to wrestle somebody for a parking spot. | 0:51:12 | 0:51:19 | |
We don't have the physical stressors most animals do. | 0:51:19 | 0:51:22 | |
We're psychologically sophisticated enough | 0:51:22 | 0:51:25 | |
to invent nonsense in our heads and we get sick with it. | 0:51:25 | 0:51:28 | |
And largely that's what baboon society is about. | 0:51:28 | 0:51:32 | |
They've got enough free time and enough food | 0:51:32 | 0:51:35 | |
that they can spend a lot of time making each other miserable | 0:51:35 | 0:51:39 | |
with social stress. | 0:51:39 | 0:51:41 | |
Here's one really stressful thing you can do if you're dominant - | 0:51:41 | 0:51:47 | |
some lower-ranking guy is in a sexual consortship with a female. | 0:51:47 | 0:51:51 | |
She's in heat, she's reproductively accessible, they're hanging out. | 0:51:51 | 0:51:55 | |
What does the dominant guy do? | 0:51:55 | 0:51:58 | |
He doesn't attack. He's just AROUND. | 0:51:58 | 0:52:00 | |
He's six feet away, just being on the scene harassing the pair. | 0:52:00 | 0:52:05 | |
No fights, no threats, no nothing. | 0:52:05 | 0:52:07 | |
The guy who's being harassed picks up and walks away voluntarily. | 0:52:07 | 0:52:11 | |
This is psychological stress, straight out of the dictionary. | 0:52:11 | 0:52:14 | |
So we know that monkeys live complex, even stressful, social lives. | 0:52:15 | 0:52:22 | |
To maintain cohesion, they constantly send and receive messages. | 0:52:22 | 0:52:26 | |
Monkeys are excellent communicators. | 0:52:30 | 0:52:33 | |
If it is possible to get inside the mind of a monkey, | 0:52:33 | 0:52:37 | |
then the squeals and chatters that accompany almost everything they do | 0:52:37 | 0:52:42 | |
perhaps hold the key. | 0:52:42 | 0:52:44 | |
Right from the beginning of these studies, | 0:52:44 | 0:52:47 | |
it was clear monkeys had something important to say to one another. | 0:52:47 | 0:52:51 | |
These vervet monkeys are warning each other of a snake. | 0:52:52 | 0:52:57 | |
RAPID CLICKING | 0:52:57 | 0:53:00 | |
The first detailed study of primate communication, | 0:53:00 | 0:53:03 | |
by Robert Seyfarth and Barbara Cheney, | 0:53:03 | 0:53:06 | |
revealed the true meaning of vervet alarm calls. | 0:53:06 | 0:53:10 | |
We did a series of experiments in 1977 and '78 | 0:53:10 | 0:53:15 | |
where we'd wait until the monkeys were foraging in a certain area | 0:53:15 | 0:53:19 | |
and then we would hide a loudspeaker in a bush nearby. | 0:53:19 | 0:53:23 | |
We'd recorded the alarm calls in natural encounters. | 0:53:23 | 0:53:27 | |
We took one of these alarm calls and played it from the loudspeaker | 0:53:27 | 0:53:32 | |
when there were no predators around. | 0:53:32 | 0:53:35 | |
The monkeys responded by running up into the trees at a leopard alarm, | 0:53:35 | 0:53:40 | |
looking up or running into bushes at an eagle alarm, | 0:53:40 | 0:53:44 | |
or looking on the ground around them in the case of a snake alarm. | 0:53:44 | 0:53:49 | |
This means that the alarm call almost functions like a word. | 0:53:49 | 0:53:54 | |
Vervet infants have to learn these words. | 0:53:56 | 0:53:59 | |
They start with broad classes of danger. | 0:53:59 | 0:54:01 | |
The eagle alarm covers all birds and flying objects. | 0:54:01 | 0:54:06 | |
As they grow, they become more precise | 0:54:08 | 0:54:12 | |
and, quite simply, those that can't learn don't survive. | 0:54:12 | 0:54:15 | |
So we now know that vervet monkeys use words | 0:54:19 | 0:54:23 | |
and have the rudiments of language. | 0:54:23 | 0:54:26 | |
Different vocalisations mean different things | 0:54:26 | 0:54:29 | |
and must be learned by the young members of the troop. | 0:54:29 | 0:54:33 | |
Recent studies have shown that that applies | 0:54:33 | 0:54:36 | |
to other species of monkeys as well. | 0:54:36 | 0:54:39 | |
And monkeys don't just call out warnings of danger. | 0:54:40 | 0:54:43 | |
Professor Mark Hauser's experiments with these rhesus macaques | 0:54:43 | 0:54:47 | |
show that they're constantly discussing food. | 0:54:47 | 0:54:52 | |
COUGH-LIKE GRUNTS | 0:54:52 | 0:54:54 | |
They tell each other about mundane food items | 0:54:54 | 0:54:58 | |
using a grunt that Mark refers to as "the boiled-potato call". | 0:54:58 | 0:55:03 | |
But they use "the caviar call" when the food is more interesting. | 0:55:08 | 0:55:12 | |
WHISTLES | 0:55:12 | 0:55:15 | |
Experiments show some monkeys even recognise the calls of other species. | 0:55:15 | 0:55:20 | |
They've become bilingual. | 0:55:20 | 0:55:23 | |
The monkeys of the Tai forest are hunted by eagles and leopards, | 0:55:30 | 0:55:35 | |
but in the wet season the greatest threat is a primate - the chimpanzee. | 0:55:35 | 0:55:40 | |
They are expert climbers that can follow monkeys into the trees | 0:55:40 | 0:55:44 | |
and they hunt cooperatively. | 0:55:44 | 0:55:46 | |
So their prey - red colobus monkeys - | 0:55:48 | 0:55:50 | |
form a partnership with Diana monkeys. | 0:55:50 | 0:55:54 | |
Klaus Zuberbuhler plays chimp calls | 0:55:56 | 0:55:59 | |
and the red colobus immediately look for Diana monkeys. | 0:55:59 | 0:56:03 | |
THEY are the most likely to spot the chimps and raise the alarm. | 0:56:03 | 0:56:07 | |
What is interesting when looking at inter-species communication, | 0:56:07 | 0:56:12 | |
between one monkey species to the next, | 0:56:12 | 0:56:16 | |
you find that each species possesses their own repertoires. | 0:56:16 | 0:56:21 | |
A Campbell's monkey has a particular alarm call for a leopard | 0:56:21 | 0:56:26 | |
which is completely different from the leopard alarm call of, say, | 0:56:26 | 0:56:30 | |
a Diana monkey | 0:56:30 | 0:56:32 | |
and it's quite obvious they do understand each other's alarm calls. | 0:56:32 | 0:56:39 | |
That tells us they must learn it at some point. | 0:56:39 | 0:56:42 | |
They aren't born with the knowledge of all these other species' calls. | 0:56:42 | 0:56:47 | |
So as it grows up, a young monkey learns the calls of other species. | 0:56:48 | 0:56:53 | |
It's a bit like learning a second language. | 0:56:53 | 0:56:56 | |
It also seems that GROUPS of calls | 0:56:57 | 0:57:00 | |
may carry a greater meaning than single ones | 0:57:00 | 0:57:03 | |
with call-groups that are equivalent to adding an adjective to a noun. | 0:57:03 | 0:57:08 | |
For example, Campbell's monkeys also have multifier calls | 0:57:08 | 0:57:13 | |
that seem to affect subsequent meaning of alarm calls | 0:57:13 | 0:57:18 | |
and it's this way of combining calls that has caught our attention. | 0:57:18 | 0:57:23 | |
And that's an important step. | 0:57:25 | 0:57:27 | |
If adding one call to another creates a new meaning, | 0:57:27 | 0:57:31 | |
there's a fascinating parallel with human language. | 0:57:31 | 0:57:34 | |
One thing is for sure - the more we listen to monkeys, | 0:57:34 | 0:57:38 | |
the more they'll tell us about their complex, highly social lives. | 0:57:38 | 0:57:43 | |
Subtitles by Red Bee Media Ltd | 0:58:16 | 0:58:19 |